Gino Camp
Erasmus University Rotterdam
Network
Latest external collaboration on country level. Dive into details by clicking on the dots.
Publication
Featured researches published by Gino Camp.
Computers in Human Behavior | 2001
Gino Camp; Fred Paas; Remy M. J. P. Rikers; Jeroen J. G. van Merriënboer
The differential effects on training and training outcome of four methods of problem selection were investigated in a computer-based training for air traffic control. In one method, training problems were given to the participants in a fixed predetermined sequence, from simple to complex. In the other methods, problems were selected dynamically, based on three different learner variables. These were mental effort, performance and mental efficiency, which is a variable that combines mental effort and performance measures to determine problem efficiency. After the training, transfer was measured. The hypothesis that dynamic problem selection would lead to more efficient training than non-dynamic problem selection was confirmed. The second hypothesis, that dynamic problem selection based on mental efficiency would lead to more efficient training and better transfer than dynamic problem selection based on performance or mental effort alone was not supported. However, the efficiency measures of the three variables were distorted by the differential effects of these variables on the acquisition phase. A possible explanation for the results is that selection based on performance stimulates rule automation, whereas selection based on mental effort or mental efficiency leads to schema acquisition.
Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition | 2007
Gino Camp; Diane Pecher; Henk G. Schmidt
Retrieval practice with particular items from memory can impair the recall of related items on a later memory test. This retrieval-induced forgetting effect has been ascribed to inhibitory processes (M. C. Anderson & B. A. Spellman, 1995). A critical finding that distinguishes inhibitory from interference explanations is that forgetting is found with independent (or extralist) cues. In 4 experiments, the authors tested whether the forgetting effect is cue-independent. Forgetting was investigated for both studied and unstudied semantically related items. Retrieval-induced forgetting was not found using item-specific independent cues for either studied or unstudied items. However, forgetting was found for both item types when studied categories were used as cues. These results are not in line with a general inhibitory account, because this account predicts retrieval-induced forgetting with independent cues. Interference and context-specific inhibition are discussed as possible explanations for the data.
Journal of Experimental Child Psychology | 2011
Anique B. H. de Bruin; Keith W. Thiede; Gino Camp; Joshua S. Redford
The ability to monitor understanding of texts, usually referred to as metacomprehension accuracy, is typically quite poor in adult learners; however, recently interventions have been developed to improve accuracy. In two experiments, we evaluated whether generating delayed keywords prior to judging comprehension improved metacomprehension accuracy for children. For sixth and seventh graders, metacomprehension accuracy was greater when generating keywords. By contrast, for fourth graders, metacomprehension accuracy did not differ across conditions. Improved metacomprehension accuracy led to improved regulation of study. The delayed keyword effect in children reported here is discussed in terms of situation model activation.
Psychonomic Bulletin & Review | 2005
Gino Camp; Diane Pecher; Henk G. Schmidt
Retrieval practice with particular items in memory may result in decreased recall of different, semantically related, items. This retrieval-induced forgetting effect has been demonstrated in studies using explicit memory tests. Anderson and Spellman (1995) have attributed retrieval-induced forgetting to inhibitory mechanisms. This hypothesis predicts similar effects in implicit memory tasks. In our first experiment, using Anderson and Spellman’s original paradigm, retrieval-induced forgetting was found using an explicit memory test with independent extralist retrieval cues. In our second experiment, using the same materials, retrieval-induced forgetting was also found using an implicit memory test with independent extralist retrieval cues, but only for participants who were aware of the relationship between the study and practice phase on the one hand, and the test phase of the experiment on the other. Thus, test awareness seems to mediate retrieval-induced forgetting in implicit memory tasks.
Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition | 2009
Gino Camp; Diane Pecher; Henk G. Schmidt; René Zeelenberg
The independent cue technique has been developed to test traditional interference theories against inhibition theories of forgetting. In the present study, the authors tested the critical criterion for the independence of independent cues: Studied cues not presented during test (and unrelated to test cues) should not contribute to the retrieval process. Participants first studied a subset of cues (e.g., rope) that were subsequently studied together with a target in a 2nd study phase (e.g., rope-sailing, sunflower-yellow). In the test phase, an extralist category cue (e.g., sports, color) was presented, and participants were instructed to recall an item from the study list that was a member of the category (e.g., sailing, yellow). The experiments showed that previous study of the paired-associate word (e.g., rope) enhanced category cued recall even though this word was not presented at test. This experimental demonstration of covert cuing has important implications for the effectiveness of the independent cue technique.
Journal of cognitive psychology | 2012
Nicole A. M. C. Goossens; Gino Camp; Peter P. J. L. Verkoeijen; Huib K. Tabbers; Rolf A. Zwaan
The spacing effect refers to the frequently observed finding that distributing learning across time leads to better retention than massing it into one single study session. In the present study, we examined whether the spacing effect generalises to primary school vocabulary learning. To this aim, children from Grade 3 were taught the meaning of 15 new words using a massed procedure and 15 other new words using a spaced procedure. The 15 words in the massed condition were divided into three sets of five words, and each set was taught three times in one of three learning sessions. In the spaced condition, learning was distributed across the three sessions: All 15 words were practised once in each of the three learning sessions. At the retention tests after 1 week and after 5 weeks we observed that the meaning of spaced words was remembered better than the meaning of massed words.
Psychological Science | 2012
Peter P. J. L. Verkoeijen; Samantha Bouwmeester; Gino Camp
Taking a memory test after an initial study phase produces better long-term retention than restudying the items, a phenomenon known as the testing effect. We propose that this effect emerges because testing strengthens semantic features of items’ memory traces, whereas restudying strengthens surface features of items’ memory traces. This novel account predicts that a testing effect should be observed even after a short retention interval when a language switch occurs between the learning phase and the final test phase. We assessed this prediction with Dutch-English bilinguals who learned Dutch Deese-Roediger-McDermott word lists through restudying or through testing (retrieval practice). Five minutes after this learning phase, they took a recognition test in Dutch (within-language condition) or in English (across-language condition). We observed a testing effect in the across-language condition, but not in the within-language condition. These findings corroborate our novel account of the testing effect.
Medical Education | 2010
Anique B. H. de Bruin; Gino Camp; Jeroen J. G. van Merriënboer
1 Holy Bible, New International Version. Proverbs 12: 15. Colorado Springs, CO: Biblica 1984. 2 Regehr G. It’s NOT rocket science: rethinking our metaphors for research in health professions education. Med Educ 2010;44:31–9. 3 Durning SJ, Artino A, Boulet J, van der Vleuten CPM, La Rochelle J, Arze B, Schuwirth L. Making use of contrasting participant views of the same encounter. Med Educ 2010;44:953–61. 4 Schuwirth LW, van der Vleuten CP. A plea for new psychometric models in educational assessment. Med Educ 2006;40:296–300. 5 Crossley J, Roberts C, Jolly B, Humphries G, Ricketts C, Norcini J, Schuwirth L. ‘I’m pickin’ up good regressions’: the governance of generalisability analyses. Med Educ 2007;41:926–34. 6 Beard JD, Purdie H, Marriott J, Crossley J. Assessing the surgical skills of trainees in the operating theatre: a prospective observational study. HTA J Ser 2010 (in press). 7 Holmboe ES, Hawkins RE, Huot SJ. Effects of training in direct observation of medical residents’ clinical competence: a randomised trial. Ann Intern Med 2004;140:874– 81.
Journal of Educational Psychology | 2001
Fred Paas; Gino Camp; Remy M. J. P. Rikers
Applied Cognitive Psychology | 2014
Nicole A. M. C. Goossens; Gino Camp; Peter P. J. L. Verkoeijen; Huib K. Tabbers