Network


Latest external collaboration on country level. Dive into details by clicking on the dots.

Hotspot


Dive into the research topics where Greg Stratmann is active.

Publication


Featured researches published by Greg Stratmann.


Anesthesiology | 2009

Isoflurane Differentially Affects Neurogenesis and Long-term Neurocognitive Function in 60-day-old and 7-day-old Rats

Greg Stratmann; Jeffrey W. Sall; Laura D. V. May; Joseph S. Bell; Kathy R. Magnusson; Visrodia Kh; Rehan S. Alvi; Ban Ku; Michael T. Lee; Ran Dai

Background:Anesthetic agents cause cell death in the developing rodent brain and long-term, mostly hippocampal-dependent, neurocognitive dysfunction. However, a causal link between these findings has not been shown. Postnatal hippocampal neurogenesis affects hippocampal function into adulthood; therefore, the authors tested the hypothesis that isoflurane affects long-term neurocognitive function via an effect on dentate gyrus neurogenesis. Methods:The S-phase marker 5-bromodeoxyuridine was administered at various times before, during, and after 4 h of isoflurane given to postnatal day (P)60 and P7 rats to assess dentate gyrus progenitor proliferation, early neuronal lineage selection, and long-term survival of new granule cell neurons. Fear conditioning and spatial reference memory was tested at various intervals from 2 weeks until 8 months after anesthesia. Results:In P60 rats, isoflurane increased early neuronal differentiation as assessed by BrdU/NeuroD costaining, decreased progenitor proliferation for 1 day, and subsequently increased progenitor proliferation 5–10 days after anesthesia. In P7 rats, isoflurane did not induce neuronal lineage selection but decreased progenitor proliferation until at least 5 days after anesthesia. Isoflurane improved spatial reference memory of P60 rats long-term, but it caused a delayed-onset, progressive, persistent hippocampal deficit in P7 rats in fear conditioning and spatial reference memory tasks. Conclusion:The authors conclude that isoflurane differentially affects both neurogenesis and long-term neurocognitive function in P60 and P7 rats. Neurogenesis might mediate the long-term neurocognitive outcome after isoflurane at different ages.


Anesthesiology | 2009

Effect of hypercarbia and isoflurane on brain cell death and neurocognitive dysfunction in 7-day-old rats.

Greg Stratmann; Laura D. V. May; Jeffrey W. Sall; Rehan S. Alvi; Joseph S. Bell; Brandi K. Ormerod; Vinuta Rau; Joan F. Hilton; Ran Dai; Michael T. Lee; Kavel H. Visrodia; Ban Ku; Emanuel J. Zusmer; Jeremy Guggenheim; Atoosa Firouzian

Background:Millions of neonates undergo anesthesia each year. Certain anesthetic agents cause brain cell death and long-term neurocognitive dysfunction in postnatal day (P)7 rats. Despite its intuitive appeal, a causal link between cell death and neurocognitive decline after anesthesia has not been established. If one existed, the degree of cell death would be expected to correlate with the degree of neurocognitive dysfunction caused by anesthesia. The authors therefore tested if cell death caused by various durations of isoflurane at 1 minimum alveolar concentration causes duration-dependent long-term neurocognitive dysfunction. Methods:Isoflurane was administered to P7 rats at 1 minimum alveolar concentration for 0, 1, 2, or 4 h. To control for the respiratory depressant effects of anesthesia, a group of rats was treated with 4 h of carbon dioxide. Cell death was assessed by FluoroJade staining 12 h after the end of each intervention, and neurocognitive outcome was assessed 8 weeks later by using fear conditioning, spatial reference memory, and spatial working memory tasks. Results:Widespread brain cell death was caused by 2 h and 4 h of isoflurane and by 4 h of carbon dioxide. The degree and distribution of thalamic cell death was similar in 4 h isoflurane-treated and 4-h carbon dioxide–treated rats. Only 4 h of isoflurane caused a long-term neurocognitive deficit affecting both spatial reference memory and spatial working memory. Working memory was improved in carbon dioxide–treated rats. Conclusion:Isoflurane-induced brain cell death may be partly caused by hypercarbia. The inconsistencies between cell death and neurocognitive outcome suggest that additional or alternative mechanisms may mediate anesthesia-induced long-term neurocognitive dysfunction.


Anesthesiology | 2012

Delayed environmental enrichment reverses sevoflurane-induced memory impairment in rats.

Shih J; Laura D. V. May; Gonzalez He; Lee Ew; Rehan S. Alvi; Jeffrey W. Sall; Philip E. Bickler; Lalchandani Gr; Yusupova M; Woodward E; Kang H; Wilk Aj; Carlston Cm; Mendoza Mv; Guggenheim Jn; Schaefer M; Rowe Am; Greg Stratmann

Background: Anesthesia given to immature rodents causes cognitive decline, raising the possibility that the same might be true for millions of children undergoing surgical procedures under general anesthesia each year. We tested the hypothesis that anesthesia-induced cognitive decline in rats is treatable. We also tested if anesthesia-induced cognitive decline is aggravated by tissue injury. Methods: Seven-day old rats underwent sevoflurane anesthesia (1 minimum alveolar concentration, 4 h) with or without tail clamping. At 4 weeks, rats were randomized to environmental enrichment or normal housing. At 8 weeks rats underwent neurocognitive testing, which consisted of fear conditioning, spatial reference memory, and water maze-based memory consolidation tests, and interrogated working memory, short-term memory, and early long-term memory. Results: Sevoflurane-treated rats had a greater escape latency when the delay between memory acquisition and memory retrieval was increased from 1 min to 1 h, indicating that short-term memory was impaired. Delayed environmental enrichment reversed the effects of sevoflurane on short-term memory and generally improved many tested aspects of cognitive function, both in sevoflurane-treated and control animals. The performance of tail-clamped rats did not differ from those rats receiving anesthesia alone. Conclusion: Sevoflurane-induced cognitive decline in rats is treatable. Delayed environmental enrichment rescued the sevoflurane-induced impairment in short-term memory. Tissue injury did not worsen the anesthesia-induced memory impairment. These findings may have relevance to neonatal and pediatric anesthesia.


Anesthesia & Analgesia | 2011

Neurotoxicity of Anesthetic Drugs in the Developing Brain

Greg Stratmann

Anesthesia kills neurons in the brain of infantile animals, including primates, and causes permanent and progressive neurocognitive decline. The anesthesia community and regulatory authorities alike are concerned that is also true in humans. In this review, I summarize what we currently know about the risks of pediatric anesthesia to long-term cognitive function. If anesthesia is discovered to cause cognitive decline in humans, we need to know how to prevent and treat it. Prevention requires knowledge of the mechanisms of anesthesia-induced cognitive decline. This review gives an overview of some of the mechanisms that have been proposed for anesthesia-induced cognitive decline and discusses possible treatment options. If anesthesia induces cognitive decline in humans, we need to know what type and duration of anesthetic is safe, and which, if any, is not safe. This review discusses early results of comparative animal studies of anesthetic neurotoxicity. Until we know if and how pediatric anesthesia affects cognition in humans, a change in anesthetic practice would be premature, not guided by evidence of better alternatives, and therefore potentially dangerous. The SmartTots initiative jointly supported by the International Anesthesia Research Society and the Food and Drug Administration aims to fund research designed to shed light on these issues that are of high priority to the anesthesia community and the public alike and therefore deserves the full support of these interest groups.


Anesthesiology | 2009

Isoflurane Inhibits Growth but Does Not Cause Cell Death in Hippocampal Neural Precursor Cells Grown in Culture

Jeffrey W. Sall; Greg Stratmann; Jason Leong; William McKleroy; Daniel Mason; Shanti Shenoy; Samuel J. Pleasure; Phillip E. Bickler

Background:Isoflurane causes long-term hippocampal-dependent learning deficits in rats despite limited isoflurane-induced hippocampal cell death, raising questions about the causality between isoflurane-induced cell death and isoflurane-induced cognitive function. Neurogenesis in the dentate gyrus is required for hippocampal-dependent learning and thus constitutes a potential alternative mechanism by which cognition can be altered after neonatal anesthesia. The authors tested the hypothesis that isoflurane alters proliferation and differentiation of hippocampal neural progenitor cells. Methods:Multipotent neural progenitor cells were isolated from pooled rat hippocampi (postnatal day 2) and grown in culture. These cells were exposed to isoflurane and evaluated for cell death using lactate dehydrogenase release, caspase activity, and immunocytochemistry for nuclear localization of cleaved caspase 3. Growth was assessed by cell counting and BrdU incorporation. Expression of markers of stemness (Sox2) and cell division (Ki67) were determined by quantitative polymerase chain reaction. Cell fate selection was assessed using immunocytochemistry to stain for neuronal and glial markers. Results:Isoflurane did not change lactate dehydrogenase release, activity of caspase 3/7, or the amount of nuclear cleaved caspase 3. Isoflurane decreased caspase 9 activity, inhibited proliferation, and decreased the proportion of cells in s-phase. messenger ribonucleic acid expression of Sox2 (stem cells) and Ki67 (proliferation) were decreased. Differentiating neural progenitor cells more often select a neuronal fate after isoflurane exposure. Conclusions:The authors conclude that isoflurane does not cause cell death, but it does act directly on neural progenitor cells independently of effects on the surrounding brain to decrease proliferation and increase neuronal fate selection. These changes could adversely affect cognition after isoflurane anesthesia.


Anesthesia & Analgesia | 2004

Reversal of direct thrombin inhibition after cardiopulmonary bypass in a patient with heparin-induced thrombocytopenia.

Greg Stratmann; Anil M. deSilva; Elaine E. Tseng; Julie Hambleton; Michel Balea; Anthony Romo; Michael J. Mann; Nancy L. Achorn; William Moskalik; Charles W. Hoopes

UNLABELLED We treated persistent hemorrhage after cardiopulmonary bypass in a heart transplant recipient who had received anticoagulation with the direct thrombin inhibitor bivalirudin by a combination therapy aimed at reducing the plasma concentration of the thrombin antagonist (hemodialysis and modified ultrafiltration), increasing the concentration of thrombin at bleeding sites (recombinant factor VIIa), and increasing the plasma concentration of other coagulation factors (fresh frozen plasma and cryoprecipitate). The bleeding was controlled, and there was no thrombotic complication. IMPLICATIONS A combination of modified ultrafiltration, hemodialysis, and the administration of recombinant factor VIIa, fresh frozen plasma, and cryoprecipitate may reverse the anticoagulant effect of bivalirudin.


Neuropsychopharmacology | 2014

Effect of General Anesthesia in Infancy on Long-Term Recognition Memory in Humans and Rats

Greg Stratmann; Joshua Lee; Jeffrey W. Sall; Bradley H. Lee; Rehan S. Alvi; Jennifer Shih; Rowe Am; Tatiana M Ramage; Flora L Chang; Terri G Alexander; David K Lempert; Nan Lin; Kasey H Siu; Sophie A Elphick; Alice Wong; Caitlin I Schnair; Alexander F Vu; John Thomas Chan; Huizhen Zai; Michelle Wong; Amanda M Anthony; Kyle C Barbour; Dana Ben-Tzur; Natalie E Kazarian; Joyce Y Y Lee; Jay R Shen; Eric Liu; Gurbir S Behniwal; Cathy R Lammers; Zoel Quinones

Anesthesia in infancy impairs performance in recognition memory tasks in mammalian animals, but it is unknown if this occurs in humans. Successful recognition can be based on stimulus familiarity or recollection of event details. Several brain structures involved in recollection are affected by anesthesia-induced neurodegeneration in animals. Therefore, we hypothesized that anesthesia in infancy impairs recollection later in life in humans and rats. Twenty eight children ages 6–11 who had undergone a procedure requiring general anesthesia before age 1 were compared with 28 age- and gender-matched children who had not undergone anesthesia. Recollection and familiarity were assessed in an object recognition memory test using receiver operator characteristic analysis. In addition, IQ and Child Behavior Checklist scores were assessed. In parallel, thirty three 7-day-old rats were randomized to receive anesthesia or sham anesthesia. Over 10 months, recollection and familiarity were assessed using an odor recognition test. We found that anesthetized children had significantly lower recollection scores and were impaired at recollecting associative information compared with controls. Familiarity, IQ, and Child Behavior Checklist scores were not different between groups. In rats, anesthetized subjects had significantly lower recollection scores than controls while familiarity was unaffected. Rats that had undergone tissue injury during anesthesia had similar recollection indices as rats that had been anesthetized without tissue injury. These findings suggest that general anesthesia in infancy impairs recollection later in life in humans and rats. In rats, this effect is independent of underlying disease or tissue injury.


Anesthesiology | 2010

Isoflurane does not affect brain cell death, hippocampal neurogenesis, or long-term neurocognitive outcome in aged rats.

Greg Stratmann; Jeffrey W. Sall; Joseph S. Bell; Rehan S. Alvi; Laura D. V. May; Ban Ku; Mitra Dowlatshahi; Ran Dai; Philip E. Bickler; Isobel A. Russell; Michael T. Lee; Margit W. Hrubos; Cheryl Chiu

Background:Roughly, 10% of elderly patients develop postoperative cognitive dysfunction. General anesthesia impairs spatial memory in aged rats, but the mechanism is not known. Hippocampal neurogenesis affects spatial learning and memory in rats, and isoflurane affects neurogenesis in neonatal and young adult rats. We tested the hypothesis that isoflurane impairs neurogenesis and hippocampal function in aged rats. Methods:Isoflurane was administered to 16-month-old rats at one minimum alveolar concentration for 4 h. FluoroJade staining was performed to assess brain cell death 16 h after isoflurane administration. Dentate gyrus progenitor proliferation was assessed by bromodeoxyuridine injection 4 days after anesthesia and quantification of bromodeoxyuridine+ cells 12 h later. Neuronal differentiation was studied by determining colocalization of bromodeoxyuridine with the immature neuronal marker NeuroD 5 days after anesthesia. New neuronal survival was assessed by quantifying cells coexpressing bromodeoxyuridine and the mature neuronal marker NeuN 5 weeks after anesthesia. Four months after anesthesia, associative learning was assessed by fear conditioning. Spatial reference memory acquisition and retention was tested in the Morris Water Maze. Results:Cell death was sporadic and not different between groups. We did not detect any differences in hippocampal progenitor proliferation, neuronal differentiation, new neuronal survival, or in any of the tests of long-term hippocampal function. Conclusion:In aged rats, isoflurane does not affect brain cell death, hippocampal neurogenesis, or long-term neurocognitive outcome.


The Annals of Thoracic Surgery | 2003

Use of recombinant factor VIIa as a rescue treatment for intractable bleeding following repeat aortic arch repair

Greg Stratmann; Isobel A. Russell; Scott Merrick

Hemorrhage, refractory to aggressive conventional therapy, at a rate of 16 L/hr following separation from cardiopulmonary bypass for aortic arch repair, was controlled with a dose of 90 microg/kg of recombinant factor VIIa, repeated once after 2 hours.


Anesthesia & Analgesia | 2006

Congenital heart disease in the adult: a review with internet-accessible transesophageal echocardiographic images.

Isobel A. Russell; Kathryn Rouine-Rapp; Greg Stratmann; Wanda C. Miller-Hance

T he number of adults recognized with congenital heart disease (CHD) has increased dramatically over the past five decades because of significant advances in diagnosis and medical and surgical care. At the moment, the population of adults with CHD (ACHD) in the United States is estimated at approximately one million (1). For the first time, the number of adults with congenital cardiovascular malformations equals the number of children with these disorders. With additional refinements in surgical techniques and definitive repair at an earlier age, this patient group is likely to increase even further. Survival rates in CHD are influenced by many factors, including year of birth, age at diagnosis, complexity of the pathology, and whether the lesion(s) has been palliated or surgically corrected (Table 1) (1). As survival and life expectancy continue to improve, a growing number of unoperated, palliated, and “repaired” individuals require surgical interventions or other procedures related or unrelated to their heart disease. The care of these patients is becoming more frequent in all surgical settings, including tertiary care facilities, ambulatory centers, and labor and delivery suites. Adults with CHD may come to the attention of anesthesiologists for various indications including:

Collaboration


Dive into the Greg Stratmann's collaboration.

Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Rehan S. Alvi

University of California

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Laura D. V. May

University of Southern California

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Jason Leong

University of California

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Bradley H. Lee

University of California

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Researchain Logo
Decentralizing Knowledge