Hiromasa Tokunaga
Osaka University
Network
Latest external collaboration on country level. Dive into details by clicking on the dots.
Publication
Featured researches published by Hiromasa Tokunaga.
Dementia and Geriatric Cognitive Disorders | 2008
Yoshihiko Kubo; Hiroaki Kazui; Tetsuihiko Yoshida; Yumiko Kito; Nobuyo Kimura; Hiromasa Tokunaga; Atsushi Ogino; Hiroji Miyake; Masatsune Ishikawa; Masatoshi Takeda
Background/Aims: We developed an idiopathic normal-pressure hydrocephalus grading scale (iNPHGS) to classify a triad of disorders (cognitive impairment, gait disturbance and urinary disturbance) of iNPH with a wide range of severity. The purpose of this study was to assess the reliability and validity of this scale in 38 patients with iNPH. Results: The interrater reliability of this scale was high. The iNPHGS cognitive domain score significantly correlated with the cognitive test scores, including the Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE), the gait domain score with the Up and Go Test and Gait Status Scale scores, and the urinary domain score with the International Consultation on Incontinence Questionnaire-Short Form (ICIQ-SF) score. The MMSE, Gait Status Scale and ICIQ-SF scores significantly improved in patients whose iNPHGS scores improved after CSF tapping but not in those whose iNPHGS scores did not improve after CSF tapping. Fourteen of the 38 patients received shunt operations. In these 14 patients, changes in the iNPHGS cognitive and urinary domains after CSF tapping were significantly associated with the changes after the shunt operation.
Dementia and Geriatric Cognitive Disorders | 2005
Hiroaki Kazui; Akemi Matsuda; Nobutsugu Hirono; Etsuro Mori; Noriko Miyoshi; Atsushi Ogino; Hiromasa Tokunaga; Yoshitaka Ikejiri; Masatoshi Takeda
We evaluated everyday memory impairment in 24 patients with mild cognitive impairment (MCI) with the Rivermead Behavioral Memory Test (RBMT) and compared the scores with those of 48 age-, sex- and education-matched normal controls (NC) and 48 age-, sex- and education-matched Alzheimer disease (AD) patients. Overall everyday memory was impaired in MCI patients but the severity was milder than that in AD patients. The MCI patients showed impairment of everyday memory tasks requiring delayed recall. But they could normally perform tasks immediately after memorizing, except for recalling and retracing a simple new route. The total Profile score correctly classified 100% of the MCI patients and 91.7% of NC, thus demonstrating the usefulness of the RBMT for diagnosing MCI patients. Prospective memory tasks were not useful for detecting the patients with MCI.
Neuroreport | 1999
Hiromasa Tokunaga; Takashi Nishikawa; Yoshitaka Ikejiri; Yoshitsugu Nakagawa; Fumihiko Yasuno; Kazuo Hashikawa; Tsunehiko Nishimura; Yoshiro Sugita; Masatoshi Takeda
To investigate the neural substrate underlying the mechanisms of Kanji and Kana writing, we conducted a PET activation study during mental writing task in eight right-handed normal Japanese subjects. During scans subjects were required to mentally write a Kanji or three Kana letters with their right hand, for each stimulant word presented auditorily. The direct comparisons between Kanji writing and Kana writing revealed that the left posterior inferior temporal gyrus was activated in Kanji writing while the left angular gyrus was activated in Kana writing. In addition, more extensive areas were activated in Kanji writing compared with Kana writing. These results suggest that different respective neural substrates are involved in Kanji and Kana writing respectively.
Dementia and Geriatric Cognitive Disorders | 2006
Atsushi Ogino; Hiroaki Kazui; Noriko Miyoshi; Mamoru Hashimoto; Shingo Ohkawa; Hiromasa Tokunaga; Yoshitaka Ikejiri; Masatoshi Takeda
Cognitive impairment in patients with idiopathic normal pressure hydrocephalous (iNPH) has not been clearly documented. We performed standardized neuropsychological assessments of 21 patients with iNPH and of 42 patients with Alzheimer’s disease (AD) matched to the patients with iNPH 2:1 by age, sex, and Mini-Mental State Examination score. Compared with the AD group, the iNPH group scored significantly higher on the orientation subtest of the Alzheimer’s Disease Assessment Scale and on the general memory and delayed recall subtests of the Wechsler Memory Scale-Revised (WMS-R), and significantly lower on the attention/concentration subtest of the WMS-R and on the digit span, arithmetic, block design and digit symbol substitution subtests of the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-Revised. The impairment of frontal lobe functions is disproportionately severe and memory impairment is disproportionately mild in patients with iNPH compared with AD. Recognition of these features contributes to an early diagnosis, which can lead to a better prognosis.
Dementia and Geriatric Cognitive Disorders | 2005
Noriko Miyoshi; Hiroaki Kazui; Atsushi Ogino; Masatsune Ishikawa; Hiroji Miyake; Hiromasa Tokunaga; Yoshitaka Ikejiri; Masatoshi Takeda
We compared the scores of the Frontal Assessment Battery (FAB), initial fluency subtest, category fluency subtest and subtests of the Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE) between patients with idiopathic normal pressure hydrocephalus (iNPH) and age-, sex- and MMSE-matched patients with Alzheimer’s disease (AD). In patients with iNPH, the time and number of steps required to go and come back a 10-meter distance were measured (Walking test) and the associations between the scores of the cognitive tests and the performance of the Walking test were evaluated. The scores of the FAB and initial fluency subtest in patients with iNPH were significantly lower than those in patients with AD. The scores of the FAB, initial fluency subtest and serial 7 subtest of the MMSE significantly correlated with the two scores of the Walking test in patients with iNPH. The present results indicate that frontal lobe functions were impaired in patients with iNPH and that cognitive impairment was closely associated with gait disturbance in patients with iNPH.
Psychogeriatrics | 2009
Hiroaki Kazui; Ryouhei Ishii; Tetsuhiko Yoshida; Koji Ikezawa; Masahiko Takaya; Hiromasa Tokunaga; Toshihisa Tanaka; Masatoshi Takeda
Charles Bonnet Syndrome (CBS) is characterized by complex formed and recurrent visual hallucinations in psychologically normal people, and is often associated with eye pathology. Many psychiatrists have taken an interest in CBS because this syndrome could provide clues to the mechanisms underlying visual hallucinations. In the present paper, we review previous neuroimaging studies in patients with CBS and summarize the results of these studies. There could be a fundamental dysfunction in the primary and secondary visual cortices in some patients with CBS, and transient cortical activation occurs in the inferior lateral temporal cortex during the appearance of visual hallucinations in CBS patients. External visual stimuli are perceived in the retina and are transmitted to the primary visual cortex (Brodmann area (BA) 17). The stimuli are transmitted from BA 17 to the secondary visual cortex (BA 18) and then to the visual association cortices (BA 19 and BA 37). In general, our perception of external visual stimuli normally has an inhibitory effect on the endogenous activation of the visual cortex. Visual loss due to certain conditions, of which eye pathology is the most commonly postulated in CBS patients, produces a state of sensory deprivation that releases the visual cortex from regulation by external stimuli, resulting in visual hallucinations (cortical release phenomenon). The results of previous neuroimaging studies suggest that the cortical release phenomenon hypothesis for the occurrence of visual hallucinations in patients with CBS is plausible. In addition, the results indicate that not only eye pathology, but also dysfunction in the primary and secondary visual cortices could result in deprivation of external visual stimuli.
Schizophrenia Research | 2009
Kazutaka Ohi; Ryota Hashimoto; Yuka Yasuda; Tetsuhiko Yoshida; Hidetoshi Takahashi; Naomi Iike; Motoyuki Fukumoto; Hironori Takamura; Masao Iwase; Kouzin Kamino; Ryouhei Ishii; Hiroaki Kazui; Ryuji Sekiyama; Yuri Kitamura; Michiyo Azechi; Koji Ikezawa; Ryu Kurimoto; Eiichiro Kamagata; Hitoshi Tanimukai; Shinji Tagami; Takashi Morihara; Masayuki Ogasawara; Hiromasa Tokunaga; Shusuke Numata; Masashi Ikeda; Tohru Ohnuma; Shu-ichi Ueno; Tomoko Fukunaga; Toshihisa Tanaka; Takashi Kudo
G72 is one of the most widely tested genes for association with schizophrenia. As G72 activates the D-amino acid oxidase (DAO), G72 is termed D-amino acid oxidase activator (DAOA). The aim of this study is to investigate the association between G72 and schizophrenia in a Japanese population, using the largest sample size to date (1774 patients with schizophrenia and 2092 healthy controls). We examined eight single nucleotide polymorphisms (SNPs), which had been associated with schizophrenia in previous studies. We found nominal evidence for association of alleles, M22/rs778293, M23/rs3918342 and M24/rs1421292, and the genotype of M22/rs778293 with schizophrenia, although there was no association of allele or genotype in the other five SNPs. We also found nominal haplotypic association, including M15/rs2391191 and M19/rs778294 with schizophrenia. However, these associations were no longer positive after correction for multiple testing. We conclude that G72 might not play a major role in the risk for schizophrenia in the Japanese population.
Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry | 2001
Takashi Nishikawa; J. Okuda; Ichiro Mizuta; K Ohno; J Jamshidi; Hiromasa Tokunaga; Yoshitaka Ikejiri; Yoshitsugu Nakagawa; Toshiki Yoshimine; Hirotaka Tanabe; Masatoshi Takeda
OBJECTIVES Three patients with callosal syndrome manifested a peculiar symptom in that they were unable to perform intended whole body actions because another intention emerged in competition with the original one. Attempts were made to clarify the symptomatology of this manifestation and its possible mechanism is discussed. METHODS The three patients are described and previous reports on patients with callosal damage were reviewed. Four additional patients with similar symptoms were found and the clinical features common to all seven patients were examined. RESULTS This symptom could not be attributed to unilateral movement disorders such as unilateral apraxia, intermanual conflict, or compulsive manipulation of tools. The manifestations included marked hesitation in initiating actions, interruption of actions, repetitive actions, and performance of unintended actions with difficulty in correcting them. All patients, except one, had a lesion in the posterior half of the body of the corpus callosum, and there was no significant involvement of the cerebral cortex. The symptom became manifest later than 4 weeks after callosal damage. It occurred during spontaneous actions, but not during well automated actions nor when following instructions. CONCLUSION This symptom, tentatively named “conflict of intentions”, can be regarded as a fragment of diagonistic dyspraxia originally described by Akelaitis, although it can occur independently of intermanual conflict. Normally, the right and left cerebral hemispheres may be complementarily modifying automated whole body actions in order to adapt behaviour to changes of the environment as well as to the intention. Partial callosal disconnection without significant cortical involvement would exaggerate the disparity between the role of each hemisphere through the reorganisation of neural systems after callosal damage. Such double, often contrary, behavioural tendencies may sometimes simultaneously enter the patients awareness.
Psychiatry Research-neuroimaging | 2000
Fumihiko Yasuno; Takashi Nishikawa; Yoshitsugu Nakagawa; Yoshitaka Ikejiri; Hiromasa Tokunaga; Ichiro Mizuta; Kazuhiro Shinozaki; Kazuo Hashikawa; Yoshiro Sugita; Tsunehiko Nishimura; Masatoshi Takeda
Psychogenic amnesia is characterized by an inability to recall information already stored in the patients memory. It is usually related to a stressful or traumatic event that cannot be explained by manifest brain damage. To examine the underlying functional disturbance of brain areas in this condition, we performed a positron emission tomography (PET) activation study on a psychogenic amnesic patient and on 12 normal control subjects. A task requiring explicit retrograde memory of faces was compared with a control task. To assess functional modifications associated with the processes of recovery, a second PET study was performed on the patient 12 months after onset. During the task, activation of the right anterior medial temporal region including the amygdala was increased in the psychogenic amnesic patient. Activation of the bilateral hippocampal regions increased only in the control subjects. During recovery, the right anterior medial temporal region became less active while the right hippocampal region became more active. Activation levels also differed in the anterior cingulate cortex, prefrontal cortex and some other cortical regions between control subjects and the patient. These findings suggest that the changes in these limbic and limbic-cortical functions are related to symptoms of the psychogenic amnesia.
Psychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences | 2005
Talant K. Doronbekov; Hiromasa Tokunaga; Yoshitaka Ikejiri; Hiroaki Kazui; Naoki Hatta; Yoshihiro Masaki; Atsushi Ogino; Noriko Miyoshi; Naohiko Oku; Takashi Nishikawa; Masatoshi Takeda
Abstract In patients with post‐traumatic stress disorder (PTSD), re‐experiencing the trauma is often induced by external cues in the environment. The cues, which were emotionally neutral for the patients before the traumatic event, become fearful ones after the event. This phenomenon is considered to be associated with fear conditioning. The paradigm was set up so that the emotionality changes in the patients with PTSD would be reproduced, and the regional cerebral blood flow (rCBF) measured with positron emission tomography (PET) was compared during exposure to the same stimuli before and after acquisition of fear conditioning. Ten healthy male subjects were asked to look at some emotionally neutral photos, then to watch a video with fearful content that also contained images similar to that presented in the photos, and afterwards to look at the photos again. Five of the 10 subjects felt that the object in the photos was more fearful after watching the video than before, and they were considered to have acquired fear conditioning. In those five subjects, the rCBF in the right amygdala and the left posterior cingulate gyrus after acquisition of fear conditioning significantly increased relative to the rCBF before conditioning. Thus, these regions seem to have a critical role in fear conditioning.