Network


Latest external collaboration on country level. Dive into details by clicking on the dots.

Hotspot


Dive into the research topics where J. Edwin Dickinson is active.

Publication


Featured researches published by J. Edwin Dickinson.


Journal of Vision | 2010

Global shape aftereffects have a local substrate: A tilt aftereffect field.

J. Edwin Dickinson; Renita A. Almeida; Jason Bell; David R. Badcock

Adaptation to prevailing stimuli is a ubiquitous property of the visual system that optimizes its dynamic range. The perceived difference in orientation of successively presented lines of similar orientation is exaggerated and the perceived shape of an object is influenced by previously experienced shapes. Change in perceived shape is assumed to arise through the adaptation of shape detectors. Here we consider an alternative: adaptation within a substrate of local oriented line detectors resulting in enhanced shape contrast in similar shapes. We show that the perceived shapes of a spatially coincident circle and Cartesian grid can be manipulated independently by adaptation to geometrically transformed copies of themselves. The same transformation was applied to the circle and the grid to create the adaptors; therefore, the specificity of the effects of adaptation demonstrates that the visual system adapts to the shape of objects rather than applying transformations to the reference frame of the visual field. The tilt aftereffect predicts local changes in perceived orientation, and fields of such local effects can often account for the global change in perceived shape of complex objects, including faces.


Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders | 2013

Visual Search Targeting Either Local or Global Perceptual Processes Differs as a Function of Autistic-Like Traits in the Typically Developing Population

Renita A. Almeida; J. Edwin Dickinson; Murray T. Maybery; Johanna C. Badcock; David R. Badcock

Relative to low scorers, high scorers on the Autism-Spectrum Quotient (AQ) show enhanced performance on the Embedded Figures Test and the Radial Frequency search task (RFST), which has been attributed to both enhanced local processing and differences in combining global percepts. We investigate the role of local and global processing further using the RFST in four experiments. High AQ adults maintained a consistent advantage in search speed across diverse target-distracter stimulus conditions. This advantage may reflect enhanced local processing of curvature in early stages of the form vision pathway and superior global detection of shape primitives. However, more probable is the presence of a superior search process that enables a consistent search advantage at both levels of processing.


Neuropsychologia | 2010

Visual search performance in the autism spectrum II: The radial frequency search task with additional segmentation cues

Renita A. Almeida; J. Edwin Dickinson; Murray T. Maybery; Johanna C. Badcock; David R. Badcock

The Embedded Figures Test (EFT) requires detecting a shape within a complex background and individuals with autism or high Autism-spectrum Quotient (AQ) scores are faster and more accurate on this task than controls. This research aimed to uncover the visual processes producing this difference. Previously we developed a search task using radial frequency (RF) patterns with controllable amounts of target/distracter overlap on which high AQ participants showed more efficient search than low AQ observers. The current study extended the design of this search task by adding two lines which traverse the display on random paths sometimes intersecting target/distracters, other times passing between them. As with the EFT, these lines segment and group the display in ways that are task irrelevant. We tested two new groups of observers and found that while RF search was slowed by the addition of segmenting lines for both groups, the high AQ group retained a consistent search advantage (reflected in a shallower gradient for reaction time as a function of set size) over the low AQ group. Further, the high AQ group were significantly faster and more accurate on the EFT compared to the low AQ group. That is, the results from the present RF search task demonstrate that segmentation and grouping created by intersecting lines does not further differentiate the groups and is therefore unlikely to be a critical factor underlying the EFT performance difference. However, once again, we found that superior EFT performance was associated with shallower gradients on the RF search task.


Journal of Vision | 2012

Further evidence that local cues to shape in RF patterns are integrated globally

J. Edwin Dickinson; Jessica McGinty; Kathryn E Webster; David R. Badcock

Radial frequency (RF) patterns, paths deformed from circular by a sinusoidal modulation of radius, have proved valuable stimuli for investigation of visual shape processing. Their utility relies upon evidence that thresholds for detection of modulation decrease, as cycles of modulation are added, at a rate that cannot be accounted for by the improving probability of detection of any single cycle (probability summation). This has been interpreted as indicative of global processing. Recently Mullen, Beaudot, and Ivanov (2011), using low contrast RF patterns viewed in cosine phase through a Gaussian window, demonstrated the existence of a local cue to modulation that was more salient than the global shape cue present in sectors of RF patterns. The experiments reported here investigate why this cue has not previously obscured global integration of shape information in RF patterns. Using stimuli modulated in sine phase, Experiment 1 showed that the presence of a circular sector of path, used to complete a partially modulated RF pattern, does not raise thresholds, contrary to the suggestion of Mullen et al. (2011). Experiment 2 demonstrated integration for high and low contrast RF patterns viewed in sine phase through a Gaussian window and Experiment 3 showed the same for patterns in cosine phase if the use of a local phase specific curvature cue was precluded. Effective use of local curvature in the test comparison, then, requires knowledge of pattern orientation to define the sign of curvature. Experiment 4 demonstrated global processing of shape information for a range of radial frequencies and also showed that the local maximum gradient with respect to circular within an RF pattern covaries with threshold. This implies that it is this cue, or one that covaries linearly with it, that is integrated across cycles of modulation by the global processing mechanism.


Vision Research | 2008

Radial frequency adaptation suggests polar-based coding of local shape cues

Jason Bell; J. Edwin Dickinson; David R. Badcock

The study of shape processing in the human visual system has frequently employed radial frequency (RF) patterns as conveniently manipulable stimuli. This study uses an adaptation paradigm to investigate how local shape information is sampled in the processing of RF contour shapes. Experiment 1 measured thresholds for detecting a fixed mean radius RF contour following adaptation to RF patterns which, in separate conditions, varied in mean radius and radial frequency. Results reveal that, adaptation is strongly tuned for RF over a range of pattern radii, but is not tuned for the number of cycles of radial modulation per visual degree of contour length; a characteristic that changes with both radius and radial frequency. Experiment 2 manipulated the polar angle separation on the fronto-parallel plane between curvature features on a fixed RF by foreshortening the pattern appearance (consistent with a rotation in depth) and shows that RF shape processing is tuned for fronto-parallel separation angles between curvature features. Results were near identical when a stereo rotation cue was added to the perspective modified RF. In the second part of Experiment 2 we showed that RF shape adaptation is also tuned for the polar angular extent of the curvature represented by the lobe at that angle. Collectively, our results indicate that the polar angle at which local curvature features appear, in addition to the angular extent of the curvature feature at that location, are both critical parameters for coding specific RF shapes.


Journal of Vision | 2013

Detecting shape change: Characterizing the interaction between texture-defined and contour-defined borders

Ken W. S. Tan; J. Edwin Dickinson; David R. Badcock

The human visual systems extreme sensitivity to subtle changes in shape can often be attributed to global pooling of local information. This has been shown for shapes described by paths of contiguous elements, but it was unknown whether this global pooling translated to shapes defined by texture-segmentation borders. Also, previous research suggests that texture and luminance cues-to-shape are integrated by the visual system for shape detection but it has not been established whether they combined for shape discrimination. Controlled shapes defined either by an explicit path of Gabors, texture-segmentation borders, or both of these cues were used. Results show that all stimuli used were globally processed. Thresholds for shapes defined by both cues matched predictions based on an independent-cue vector sum of individual thresholds. Thus, while local elements are integrated around the contour and are processed by global shape-detection mechanisms, integration did not occur across different shape-cues.


Journal of Vision | 2015

The broad orientation dependence of the motion streak aftereffect reveals interactions between form and motion neurons.

Matthew F. Tang; J. Edwin Dickinson; Troy A. W. Visser; David R. Badcock

The extended integration time of visual neurons can lead to the production of the neural equivalent of an orientation cue along the axis of motion in response to fast-moving objects. The dominant model argues that these motion streaks resolve the inherent directional uncertainty arising from the small size of receptive fields in V1, by combining spatial orientation with motion signals in V1. This model was tested in humans using visual aftereffects, in which adapting to a static grating causes the perceived direction of a subsequently presented motion stimulus to be tilted away from the adapting orientation. We found that a much broader range of orientations produced aftereffects than predicted by the current model, suggesting that these orientation cues influence motion perception at a later stage than V1. We also found that varying the spatial frequency of the adaptor changed the aftereffect from repulsive to attractive for motion-test but not form-test stimuli. Finally, manipulations of V1 excitability, using transcranial stimulation, reduced the aftereffect, suggesting that the orientation cue is dependent on V1. These results can be accounted for if the orientation information from the motion streak, gathered in V1, enters the motion system at a later stage of motion processing, most likely V5. A computational model of motion direction is presented incorporating gain modifications of broadly tuned motion-selective neurons by narrowly tuned orientation-selective cells in V1, which successfully accounts for the extant data. These results reinforce the suggestion that orientation places strong constraints on motion processing but in a previously undescribed manner.


Vision Research | 2009

Second-order orientation cues to the axis of motion

David R. Badcock; J. Edwin Dickinson

Geislers [Geisler, W. S. (1999). Motion streaks provide a spatial code for motion direction. Nature, 400, 65-69] model of motion processing proposes that image smear arising from extended integration periods in Simple cells creates motion streaks which indicate the axis of motion. Orientation cues were provided using textured dot-pairs composed of randomly-placed luminance increments and decrements giving contours with the same average luminance as the background and incompatible with smear. These contours were equally effective in signalling both motion axis and coherence. The results support the assertion that extended contours can determine the perceived axis of motion and that the motion system can use second-order texture cues for this purpose. Inputs of this type are therefore required for both Geislers (1999) and Barlow and Olshausens [Barlow, H. B., & Olshausen, B. A. (2004). Convergent evidence for the visual analysis of optic flow through anisotropic attenuation of high spatial frequencies. Journal of Vision, 4, 415-426] models of this ability.


PLOS ONE | 2013

Near Their Thresholds for Detection, Shapes Are Discriminated by the Angular Separation of Their Corners

J. Edwin Dickinson; Jason Bell; David R. Badcock

Observers make sense of scenes by parsing images on the retina into meaningful objects. This ability is retained for line drawings, demonstrating that critical information is concentrated at object boundaries. Information theoretic studies argue for further concentration at points of maximum curvature, or corners, on such boundaries [1]–[3] suggesting that the relative positions of such corners might be important in defining shape. In this study we use patterns subtly deformed from circular, by a sinusoidal modulation of radius, in order to measure threshold sensitivity to shape change. By examining the ability of observers to discriminate between patterns of different frequency and/or number of cycles of modulation in a 2x2 forced choice task we were able to show, psychophysically, that difference in a single cue, the periodicity of the corners (specifically the polar angle between two points of maximum curvature) was sufficient to allow discrimination of two patterns near their thresholds for detection. We conclude that patterns could be considered as labelled for this measure. These results suggest that a small number of such labels might be sufficient to identify an object.


Journal of Vision | 2009

Selective attention contributes to global processing in vision

J. Edwin Dickinson; Cherese Broderick; David R. Badcock

Information processing is more effective within attended regions of the visual field and the size of the attended region is variable. This observation conflicts with the assumption, used in measuring the spatial extent of global integration of coherent local orientations, that optimal sensitivity to texture information is immediately available within an appropriately sized neuronal receptive field. Using extended patterns that require global processing to detect the presence of coherent orientation structure, we found that the size and topology of the region of integration of local visual cues is not fixed. Integration can occur out to a radius of at least 10 degrees, an area (314 square degrees) much larger than previously supposed, and can be constrained to annular in addition to circular apertures. The use of such spatial apertures was found to be mediated by observer expectation. The processing of texture information available in selected areas is optimized through the exclusion of noise outside the regions of interest.

Collaboration


Dive into the J. Edwin Dickinson's collaboration.

Top Co-Authors

Avatar

David R. Badcock

University of Western Australia

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Johanna C. Badcock

University of Western Australia

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Renita A. Almeida

University of Western Australia

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Murray T. Maybery

University of Western Australia

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Ken W. S. Tan

University of Western Australia

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Jason Bell

University of Western Australia

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Kathryn E Webster

University of Western Australia

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Matthew F. Tang

University of Western Australia

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Robert J. Green

University of Western Australia

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Researchain Logo
Decentralizing Knowledge