James C. Thompson
Swinburne University of Technology
Network
Latest external collaboration on country level. Dive into details by clicking on the dots.
Publication
Featured researches published by James C. Thompson.
NeuroImage | 2004
Kylie J. Wheaton; James C. Thompson; Ari Syngeniotis; David F. Abbott; Aina Puce
Activation of premotor and temporoparietal cortex occurs when we observe others movements, particularly relating to objects. Viewing the motion of different body parts without the context of an object has not been systematically evaluated. During a 3T fMRI study, 12 healthy subjects viewed human face, hand, and leg motion, which was not directed at or did not involve an object. Activation was identified relative to static images of the same human face, hand, and leg in both individual subject and group average data. Four clear activation foci emerged: (1) right MT/V5 activated to all forms of viewed motion; (2) right STS activated to face and leg motion; (3) ventral premotor cortex activated to face, hand, and leg motion in the right hemisphere and to leg motion in the left hemisphere; and (4) anterior intraparietal cortex (aIP) was active bilaterally to viewing hand motion and in the right hemisphere leg motion. In addition, in the group data, a somatotopic activation pattern for viewing face, hand, and leg motion occurred in right ventral premotor cortex. Activation patterns in STS and aIP were more complex--typically activation foci to viewing two types of human motion showed some overlap. Activation in individual subjects was similar; however, activation to hand motion also occurred in the STS with a variable location across subjects--explaining the lack of a clear activation focus in the group data. The data indicate that there are selective responses to viewing motion of different body parts in the human brain that are independent of object or tool use.
NeuroImage | 2003
Aina Puce; Ari Syngeniotis; James C. Thompson; David F. Abbott; Kylie J. Wheaton; Umberto Castiello
Physiological studies in humans and monkeys indicate that the posterior temporal cortex is active when viewing the movements of others. Here we tested the premise that this region integrates form and motion information by presenting both natural and line-drawn displays of moving faces and motion controls where motion was continuously presented in the same part of the visual field. The cortex in and near the STS and on the fusiform gyrus (FG) responded to both types of face stimuli, but not to the controls, in a functional magnetic resonance imaging study in 10 normal subjects. The response in the STS to both types of facial motion was equal in magnitude, whereas in the FG the natural image of the face produced a significantly greater response than that of the line-drawn face. In a subsequent recording session, the electrical activity of the brain was recorded in the same subjects to the same activation task. Significantly larger event-related potentials (ERPs) to both types of moving faces were observed over the posterior temporal scalp compared to the motion controls at around 200 ms postmotion onset. Taken together, these data suggest that regions of temporal cortex actively integrate form and motion information-a process largely independent of low-level visual processes such as changes in local luminance and contrast.
Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience | 2008
Jillian E. Hardee; James C. Thompson; Aina Puce
The detection of threat is a role that the amygdala plays well, evidenced by its increased response to fearful faces in human neuroimaging studies. A critical element of the fearful face is an increase in eye white area (EWA), hypothesized to be a significant cue in activating the amygdala. However, another important social signal that can increase EWA is a lateral shift in gaze direction, which also serves to orient attention to potential threats. It is unknown how the amygdala differentiates between these increases in EWA and those that are specifically associated with fear. Using functional magnetic resonance imaging, we show that the left amygdala distinguished between fearful eyes and gaze shifts despite similar EWA increases whereas the right amygdala was less discriminatory. Additional analyses also revealed selective hemispheric response patterns in the left fusiform gyrus. Our data show clear hemispheric differences in EWA-based fear activation, suggesting the existence of parallel mechanisms that code for emotional face information.
NeuroImage | 2007
James C. Thompson; Jillian E. Hardee; Anita Panayiotou; David P. Crewther; Aina Puce
The superior temporal sulcus (STS) and surrounding lateral temporal and inferior parietal cortices are an important part of a network involved in the processing of biological movement. It is unclear whether the STS responds to the movement of different body parts uniformly, or if the response depends on the body part that is moving. Here we examined brain activity to recognizing sequences of face and hand movements as well as radial grating motion, controlling for differences in movement dynamics between stimuli. A region of the right posterior STS (pSTS) showed common activation to both face and hand motion, relative to radial grating motion, with no significant difference between responses to face and hand motion in this region. Distinct responses to face motion relative to hand motion were observed in the right mid-STS, while the right posterior inferior temporal sulcus (pITS) and inferior parietal lobule (IPL) showed greater responses to hand motion relative to face motion. These findings indicate that while there may be distinct processing of different body part motion in lateral temporal and inferior parietal cortices, the response of the pSTS is not body part specific. This region may provide input to other parts of a network involved with processing human actions with a high-level visual description of biological motion.
Clinical Neurophysiology | 2000
James C. Thompson; Katherine Tzambazis; Con Stough; Ken Nagata; Richard B. Silberstein
OBJECTIVES The high alpha/low beta range of the spontaneous EEG appears to be particularly sensitive to the effects of nicotine. The present study examined the acute effects of nicotine on the topography of the 13 Hz steady-state visually evoked potential (SSVEP). METHODS Thirteen moderate smokers participated in a repeated-measures design. The amplitude and latency of the SSVEP elicited by an unstructured sinusoidal 13 Hz flicker following a <0. 05 mg nicotine cigarette were compared to those following a 0.8 mg nicotine cigarette. RESULTS The nicotine condition was associated with an increase in the amplitude of the SSVEP, when compared to the placebo condition, and this increase was greatest in central and right parietal regions. The latency of the SSVEP was reduced in the nicotine condition in bilateral frontal and right parietal regions. CONCLUSIONS These results are similar to the effects of nicotine seen in studies examining spontaneous EEG, and are consistent with other studies indicating that the 13 Hz SSVEP indexes brain electrical activity in the high alpha/low beta range. The findings are discussed in terms of possible functional significance of nicotine-induced cortical activation in this frequency range.
Intelligence | 2001
Con Stough; James C. Thompson; Timothy C. Bates; Pradeep J. Nathan
Inspection time (IT), an information-processing correlate of psychometric intelligence, has been extensively studied. Previous research has shown that IT is a reliable correlate of psychometric intelligence across different developmental periods, mirroring developmental trends of fluid intelligence. Despite this extensive previous literature, very little is known about the biological basis of IT. In the present review, we discuss recent results from our laboratories examining the neurochemical determinants of IT. In this review, we outline the significance of several studies in which performance on the IT task is measured before and after modulating key human central nervous system (CNS) neurotransmitters and receptor systems (e.g., cholinergic, serotonergic, noradrenergic, and dopaminergic systems). The results of these studies indicate a primarily cholinergic basis for IT, although other aspects of psychometric intelligence may have serotonergic and dopaminergic determinants in addition to a cholinergic basis. The results are consistent with data reporting cholinergic depletion and impaired IT performance in dementia of the Alzheimers type (DAT). Speculatively, we propose that compounds that enhance the release of the neurotransmitter acetylcholine (Ach) will improve IT and the variance that IT shares with IQ test performance.
Neuropsychologia | 2004
Andrew M. Johnson; Quincy J. Almeida; Con Stough; James C. Thompson; Rene Singarayer; Mandar Jog
Psychopharmacology | 2000
James C. Thompson; Con Stough; David Ames; C. Ritchie; Pradeep J. Nathan
Cognitive Brain Research | 2004
James C. Thompson; David F. Abbott; Kylie J. Wheaton; Ari Syngeniotis; Aina Puce
Human Psychopharmacology-clinical and Experimental | 2002
James C. Thompson; Georgina Wilby; Con Stough