Network


Latest external collaboration on country level. Dive into details by clicking on the dots.

Hotspot


Dive into the research topics where Janny C. Stapel is active.

Publication


Featured researches published by Janny C. Stapel.


Psychological Research-psychologische Forschung | 2012

Online prediction of others’ actions: the contribution of the target object, action context and movement kinematics

Janny C. Stapel; Sabine Hunnius; Harold Bekkering

Previous research investigated the contributions of target objects, situational context and movement kinematics to action prediction separately. The current study addresses how these three factors combine in the prediction of observed actions. Participants observed an actor whose movements were constrained by the situational context or not, and object-directed or not. After several steps, participants had to indicate how the action would continue. Experiment 1 shows that predictions were most accurate when the action was constrained and object-directed. Experiments 2A and 2B investigated whether these predictions relied more on the presence of a target object or cues in the actor’s movement kinematics. The target object was artificially moved to another location or occluded. Results suggest a crucial role for kinematics. In sum, observers predict actions based on target objects and situational constraints, and they exploit subtle movement cues of the observed actor rather than the direct visual information about target objects and context.


Journal of cognitive psychology | 2015

The development of numerosity estimation: Evidence for a linear number representation early in life

Janny C. Stapel; Sabine Hunnius; Harold Bekkering; Oliver Lindemann

Several studies investigating the development of approximate number representations used the number-to-position task and reported evidence for a shift from a logarithmic to a linear representation of numerical magnitude with increasing age. However, this interpretation as well as the number-to-position method itself has been questioned recently. The current study tested 5- and 8-year-old children on a newly established numerosity production task to examine developmental changes in number representations and to test the idea of a representational shift. Modelling of the childrens numerical estimations revealed that responses of the 8-year-old children approximate a simple positive linear relation between estimated and actual numbers. Interestingly, however, the estimations of the 5-year-old children were best described by a bilinear model reflecting a relatively accurate linear representation of small numbers and no apparent magnitude knowledge for large numbers. Taken together, our findings provide no support for a shift of mental representations from a logarithmic to a linear metric but rather suggest that the range of number words which are appropriately conceptualised and represented by linear analogue magnitude codes expands during development.


Frontiers in Psychology | 2016

Young Children's Motor Interference Is Influenced by Novel Group Membership.

Johanna E. van Schaik; Hinke M. Endedijk; Janny C. Stapel; Sabine Hunnius

From early childhood onward, individuals use behavior copying to communicate liking and belonging. This non-verbal signal of affiliation is especially relevant in the context of social groups and indeed both children and adults copy in-group more than out-group members. Given the societal importance of inter-group interactions, it is imperative to understand the mechanistic level at which group modulations of copying occur early in development. The current study was designed to investigate the effect of novel group membership on young children’s motor behavior during a simultaneous movement-observation and -execution task. Four- to six-year-olds (n = 65) first gained membership to one of two novel groups based on their color preference and put on a vest in their chosen color. Subsequently, they were instructed to draw a straight line back-and-forth on a tablet computer that was concurrently displaying a stimulus video in which a model moved her arm congruently or incongruently to the child’s instructed direction. In half of the stimulus videos the model belonged to the in-group, while in the other half the model belonged to the out-group, as identified by the color of her dress. The deviations into the uninstructed direction of the children’s drawings were quantified as a measure of how much observing the models’ behaviors interfered with executing their own behaviors. The motor interference effect, namely higher deviations in the incongruent trials than in the congruent trials, was found only for the out-group condition. An additional manipulation of whether the models’ arms followed a biological or non-biological velocity profile had little effect on children’s motor interference. The results are interpreted in the context of the explicit coordinative nature of the task as an effect of heightened attention toward interacting with an out-group member. This study demonstrates that already during early childhood, novel group membership dynamically influences behavior processing as a function of interaction context.


Frontiers in Psychology | 2015

Fifteen-month-old infants use velocity information to predict others' action targets

Janny C. Stapel; Sabine Hunnius; Harold Bekkering

In a world full of objects, predicting which object a person is going to grasp is not easy for an onlooker. Among other cues, the characteristics of a reaching movement might be informative for predicting its target, as approach movements are slower when more accuracy is required. The current study examined whether observers can predict the target of an action based on the movement velocity while the action is still unfolding, and if so, whether these predictions are likely the result of motor simulation. We investigated the role of motor processes for velocity-based predictions by studying participants who based on their age differed in motor experience with the task at hand, namely reaching. To that end, 9-, 12-, and 15-month-old infants and a group of adults participated in an eye-tracking experiment which assessed action prediction accuracy. Participants observed a hand repeatedly moving toward and pressing a button on a panel, one of which was small, the other one large. The velocity of the reaching hand was the central cue for predicting which button would be the target of the observed action as the velocity was lower when reaching for the small compared to the large button. Adults and 15-month-old infants made more frequent visual anticipations to the close button when it was the target than when it was not and were thus able to use the information in the speed of the approach movement for the prediction of the action target. The 9- and 12-month-olds, however, did not display this difference. After the eye-tracking experiment, infants’ ability to aim for and press buttons of different sizes was evaluated. Results showed that the 15-month-olds were more proficient than the 9- and 12-month-olds in performing the reaching actions. The developmental time line of velocity-based action predictions thus corresponds to the development of performing that motor act yourself. Taken together, these data suggest that motor simulation may underlie velocity-based predictions.


Psychological Research-psychologische Forschung | 2017

The role of head and hand movements for infants’ predictions of others’ actions

Benjamin Koch; Janny C. Stapel

In everyday life, both the head and the hand movements of another person reveal the other’s action target. However, studies on the development of action prediction have primarily included displays in which only hand and no head movements were visible. Given that infants acquire in their first year both the ability to follow other’s gaze and the ability to predict other’s reaching actions, the question is whether they rely mostly on the hand or the head when predicting other’s manual actions. The current study aimed to provide an answer to this question using a screen-based eye tracking setup. Thirteen-month-old infants observed a model transporting plastic rings from one side of the screen to the other side and place them on a pole. In randomized trials the model’s head was either visible or occluded. The dependent variable was gaze-arrival time, which indicated whether participants predicted the model’s action targets. Gaze-arrival times were not found to be different when the head was visible or rendered invisible. Furthermore, target looks that occurred after looks at the hand were found to be predictive, whereas target looks that occurred after looks at the head were reactive. In sum, the study shows that 13-month-olds are capable of predicting an individual’s action target based on the observed hand movements but not the head movements. The data suggest that earlier findings on infants’ action prediction in screen-based tasks in which often only the hands were visible may well generalize to real-life settings in which infants have visual access to the actor’s head.


Social Neuroscience | 2010

Motor activation during observation of unusual versus ordinary actions in infancy

Janny C. Stapel; Sabine Hunnius; Michiel van Elk; Harold Bekkering


Cognition | 2016

Motor system contribution to action prediction: Temporal accuracy depends on motor experience

Janny C. Stapel; Sabine Hunnius; Marlene Meyer; Harold Bekkering


British Journal of Development Psychology | 2016

Monitoring others' errors: The role of the motor system in early childhood and adulthood

Marlene Meyer; Ricarda Braukmann; Janny C. Stapel; Harold Bekkering; Sabine Hunnius


Journal of Experimental Child Psychology | 2015

The role of action prediction and inhibitory control for joint action coordination in toddlers

Marlene Meyer; Harold Bekkering; R. Haartsen; Janny C. Stapel; Sabine Hunnius


Donders Graduate Schoolfor Cognitive Neuroscience series ; 189 | 2015

Action prediction and the development thereof

Janny C. Stapel

Collaboration


Dive into the Janny C. Stapel's collaboration.

Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Sabine Hunnius

Radboud University Nijmegen

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Harold Bekkering

Radboud University Nijmegen

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Marlene Meyer

Radboud University Nijmegen

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Ricarda Braukmann

Radboud University Nijmegen

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Hinke M. Endedijk

Radboud University Nijmegen

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Ilse van Wijk

Radboud University Nijmegen

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

J. van Hoof

The Hague University of Applied Sciences

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Researchain Logo
Decentralizing Knowledge