Jennifer Highsmith
University of Nevada, Reno
Network
Latest external collaboration on country level. Dive into details by clicking on the dots.
Publication
Featured researches published by Jennifer Highsmith.
Visual Neuroscience | 2004
Michael A. Crognale; Michael F Fry; Jennifer Highsmith; Gunilla Haegerstrom-Portnoy; Maureen Neitz; Jay Neitz; Michael A. Webster
X-linked incomplete achromatopsia (XIA), also called blue-cone monochromacy (BCM), is a rare cone disorder that most commonly results either from one of two conditions. The first condition is a deletion of the locus control region (LCR) which is a critical DNA element that lies upstream of the L and M photopigment gene array on the X-chromosome and is necessary for expression of the photopigment genes. The second condition is an inactivating point mutation within the coding sequence of the remaining photopigment gene in an array from which all but one gene has been deleted. Many previous studies have concluded that affected individuals either have only rods and S-cones (Blackwell & Blackwell, 1957, 1961; Daw & Enoch, 1973; Hess et al., 1989) or have rods, S-cones, and another cone type that contains the rod pigment (Pokorny et al., 1970; Alpern et al., 1971). However, Smith et al. (1983) described individuals with XIA who had residual L-cone function. Here we report results for a subject with XIA who appears to have residual M-cone function. Genetic analysis revealed that he had apparently normal genes for M-cone photopigment thus leaving open the possibility that he has a contribution to vision based on expression of these genes at a very low level.
Ophthalmic and Physiological Optics | 2010
Jennifer Highsmith; Michael A. Crognale
Attention is important for sufficient performance on many visual tasks. This has been shown using achromatic steady‐state and pattern‐reversal VEPs. Waveform characteristics typically attenuate when attending to distractor stimuli and ignoring VEP stimuli. Chromatic pattern‐onset responses have not been tested under conditions of selective attention: as they can be used in clinical settings to test color vision, it is important to know what effects attentional shifts would have on this response. In the present study chromatic pattern‐onset VEPs were recorded using spatially divided and spatially contiguous VEP and distractor stimuli. VEP stimuli were 1 cycle.deg−1 horizontal sine wave patterns (onset mode 100 ms on/400 ms off) used to selectively modulate the L−M and S‐(L+M) visual pathways. Distracter stimuli were letters. Subjects attended to either the letters or the gratings and pressed a button when a predetermined stimulus appeared. In Experiment one, VEP and distractor stimuli were superimposed and spatially contiguous. In Experiment two, stimuli were presented to different hemifields. No significant changes in waveform amplitude and latency were found between VEP and distractor attention conditions for either visual pathway. For the chromatic pattern‐onset response, modulation of attention does not change responses either with spatially contiguous or spatially separate selective attention manipulations. Consequently, it may not be necessary to monitor attention during recording of this response.
Journal of Vision | 2010
Jennifer Highsmith; David Stoebling; Peter Gulla; Michael A. Crognale
Documenta Ophthalmologica | 2009
Jennifer Highsmith; Michael A. Crognale
Journal of Vision | 2005
Sarah L. Elliott; Jennifer Highsmith; Samuel D. Crognale; Michael A. Crognale
Journal of Vision | 2010
Jennifer Highsmith; Zenaida Santiago; Michael A. Crognale
Journal of Vision | 2010
Jennifer Highsmith; Chad S. Duncan; Sean F. O'Neil; Eric Roth; Michael A. Crognale
Journal of Vision | 2010
Michael F Fry; Jennifer Highsmith; Michael A. Crognale
Journal of Vision | 2010
Sarah L. Elliott; Eric Roth; Jennifer Highsmith; John S. Werner; Michael A. Webster
Journal of Vision | 2005
Jennifer Highsmith; Michael A. Crognale