Network


Latest external collaboration on country level. Dive into details by clicking on the dots.

Hotspot


Dive into the research topics where Jo-Anne LeFevre is active.

Publication


Featured researches published by Jo-Anne LeFevre.


Child Development | 2002

Parental Involvement in the Development of Children's Reading Skill: A Five-Year Longitudinal Study

Monique Sénéchal; Jo-Anne LeFevre

This article presents the findings of the final phase of a 5-year longitudinal study with 168 middle- and upper middle-class children in which the complex relations among early home literacy experiences, subsequent receptive language and emergent literacy skills, and reading achievement were examined. Results showed that childrens exposure to books was related to the development of vocabulary and listening comprehension skills, and that these language skills were directly related to childrens reading in grade 3. In contrast, parent involvement in teaching children about reading and writing words was related to the development of early literacy skills. Early literacy skills directly predicted word reading at the end of grade 1 and indirectly predicted reading in grade 3. Word reading at the end of grade 1 predicted reading comprehension in grade 3. Thus, the various pathways that lead to fluent reading have their roots in different aspects of childrens early experiences.


European Journal of Cognitive Psychology | 2004

The role of working memory in mental arithmetic

Diana DeStefano; Jo-Anne LeFevre

We reviewed the literature on the role of working memory in the solution of arithmetic problems such as 3 + 4 or 345 + 29. The literature was neither comprehensive nor systematic, but a few conclusions are tenable. First, all three components of the working memory system proposed by Baddeley (i.e., central executive, phonological loop, and visual‐spatial sketchpad) play a role in mental arithmetic, albeit under different conditions. Second, mental arithmetic requires central executive resources, even for single‐digit problems. Third, further progress in understanding the role of working memory in arithmetic requires that researchers systematically manipulate factors such as presentation conditions (e.g., operand duration, format), problem complexity, task requirements (e.g., verification vs production), and response requirements (e.g., spoken vs written); and that they consider individual differences in solution procedures. Fourth, the encoding‐complex model (Campbell, 1994) seems more likely to account for the variability observed in arithmetic solutions than other models of numerical processing. Finally, working memory researchers are urged to use mental arithmetic as a primary task because the results of the present review suggest that solution of problems that involve multiple digits are likely to involve an interaction of all the components of the working memory system.


Child Development | 2010

Pathways to Mathematics: Longitudinal Predictors of Performance.

Jo-Anne LeFevre; Lisa Fast; Sheri-Lynn Skwarchuk; Brenda Smith-Chant; Jeffrey Bisanz; Deepthi Kamawar; Marcie Penner-Wilger

A model of the relations among cognitive precursors, early numeracy skill, and mathematical outcomes was tested for 182 children from 4.5 to 7.5 years of age. The model integrates research from neuroimaging, clinical populations, and normal development in children and adults. It includes 3 precursor pathways: quantitative, linguistic, and spatial attention. These pathways (a) contributed independently to early numeracy skills during preschool and kindergarten and (b) related differentially to performance on a variety of mathematical outcomes 2 years later. The success of the model in accounting for performance highlights the need to understand the fundamental underlying skills that contribute to diverse forms of mathematical competence.


Computers in Human Behavior | 2007

Cognitive load in hypertext reading: A review

Diana DeStefano; Jo-Anne LeFevre

A process model of hypertext reading was used to generate predictions about the effects of hypertext features on cognitive processing during text navigation and comprehension. We evaluated the predictions of the model with respect to the extant literature, focusing on studies in which versions of hypertexts were compared. Consistent with our predictions, the increased demands of decision-making and visual processing in hypertext impaired reading performance. Individual differences in readers, such as working memory capacity and prior knowledge, mediated the impact of hypertext features. For example, readers with low working memory and low prior knowledge were usually disadvantaged in hypertext. Some benefits were observed for learners with low prior knowledge, however, if the hypertext structure was hierarchical and consistent with that of the knowledge domain. We also surveyed the effectiveness of structural features designed to reduce cognitive load, including graphical overviews, restricted access to links, and visible link types. Complex graphical overviews did not reliably enable learning and navigation, whereas navigational support from restricted access and visible link types were helpful. We identified gaps in the empirical literature and suggested future studies to investigate cognitive processes in hypertext reading.


Journal of School Psychology | 2001

On Refining Theoretical Models of Emergent Literacy The Role of Empirical Evidence

Monique Sénéchal; Jo-Anne LeFevre; Brenda Smith-Chant; Karen V Colton

Abstract Childrens emergent literacy has received considerable attention in the last decade. The modal view of emergent literacy is that it encompasses all aspects of childrens oral and written language skills. The present article proposes an alternative view whereby emergent literacy is a separate construct from oral language and metalinguistic skills. It is also proposed that emergent literacy is composed of two distinct components; childrens conceptual knowledge (e.g., knowledge of the functions of print) and childrens early procedural knowledge of writing and reading (e.g., invented spelling). Evidence is presented that supports this differentiated view of language and emergent literacy by showing that distinct patterns of relations exist among emergent literacy, oral language, and metalinguistic skills. It is concluded that separating the constructs of language and emergent literacy is an interesting alternative to current conceptions of emergent literacy. In time, such theoretical fine tuning will serve as better guides for policy and practice.


Memory & Cognition | 1988

Cognitive arithmetic: evidence for obligatory activation of arithmetic facts.

Jo-Anne LeFevre; Jeffrey Bisanz; Linda Mrkonjic

In two experiments, obligatory activation of arithmetic facts resulted in interference on a simple number-matching task. Subjects were required to verify the presence of a probe number (e.g., 5) in a previously presented pair (e.g., 5+1). Items for which the probe was the sum of the initial pair (e.g., 5+1 and 6) were rejected more slowly than items for which the probe was not the sum (e.g., 5+1 and 3), and this effect was largest at stimulus onset asynchronies of less than 180 msec between the number pair and the probe. The results are consistent with the notion that arithmetic knowledge is represented in an associative network and accessed by means of spreading activation.


Memory & Cognition | 2003

Phonological and visual working memory in mental addition

Patricia L. Trbovich; Jo-Anne LeFevre

The goal of the present research was to examine the role of working memory in mental arithmetic. Adults (n = 96) solved multidigit arithmetic problems (e.g., 52 + 3; 3 + 52) alone and in combination with either a phonological memory load (i.e., nonwords, such asgup) or a visual memory load (i.e., random pattern of asterisks). The participants solved problems presented in a vertical format significantly faster than problems presented in a horizontal format. They also solved double digit first problems (e.g., 52 + 3) more quickly than the reverse (e.g., 3 + 52), but only when the problems were presented horizontally. Performance was worse in the phonological load condition than in the visual load condition for the participants who solved problems presented horizontally, whereas performance was worse in the visual load condition than in the phonological load condition when problems were presented vertically. The present research provides evidence that both phonological and visual aspects of working memory are involved in mental arithmetic but that the role of each working memory component will depend on such factors as presentation format.


International Journal of Early Years Education | 2010

Do home numeracy and literacy practices of Greek and Canadian parents predict the numeracy skills of kindergarten children

Jo-Anne LeFevre; Eleoussa Polyzoi; Sheri-Lynn Skwarchuk; Lisa Fast; Carla Sowinski

Children’s experiences with early numeracy and literacy activities are a likely source of individual differences in their preparation for academic learning in school. What factors predict differences in children’s experiences? We hypothesised that relations between parents’ practices and children’s numeracy skills would mediate the relations between numeracy skills and parents’ education, attitudes and expectations. Parents of Greek (N = 100) and Canadian (N = 104) five‐year‐old children completed a survey about parents’ home practices, academic expectations and attitudes; their children were tested on two numeracy measures (i.e., KeyMath‐Revised Numeration and next number generation). Greek parents reported numeracy and literacy activities less frequently than Canadian parents; however, the frequency of home numeracy activities that involved direct experiences with numbers or mathematical content (e.g., learning simple sums, mental math) was related to children’s numeracy skills in both countries. For Greek children, home literacy experiences (i.e., storybook exposure) also predicted numeracy outcomes. The mediation model was supported for Greek children, but for Canadian children, the parent factors had both direct and mediated relations with home practices.


Memory & Cognition | 1999

More on the relation between division and multiplication in simple arithmetic: evidence for mediation of division solutions via multiplication.

Jo-Anne LeFevre; Jason Morris

Adults (N=32) solved simple multiplication (e.g., 8×7) and corresponding division problems (e.g., 56/8). Self-reports of solution processes were given by half of the participants. Latency patterns and error rates were closely related across operations and were similar in self-report and no-report conditions. Solution of division problems, however, facilitated solution of multiplication problems more than the reverse. On large division problems, participants reported that they “recast” problems as multiplication (e.g., 56/8 as 8×?=56). These results support the hypothesis that multiplication and division are stored in separate mental representations but that solution of difficult division problems sometimes involves access to multiplication.


Mathematical Cognition | 1997

The Role of Experience in Numerical Skill: Multiplication Performance in Adults from Canada and China.

Jo-Anne LeFevre; Jing Liu

Adults educated either in the People s Republic of China or in Canada solved single-digit multiplication problems. Chinese adults were faster and made fewer errors than Canadian adults and showed smaller increases in latency and errors as problem size increased. Chinese adults transformed problems with the larger operand on the left e.g. 9 6 by reversing the digits and solving the complementary problem e.g. 6 9. Only the Canadian adults showed a substantial advantage in latencies and errors for problems with operands of 5. Although both groups showed a latency advantage on ties e.g. 3 3 as compared to other problems, the advantage was much larger for the Canadian than for the Chinese adults. These findings were only partially attributable to overall differences in skill; patterns of differences persisted when groups were equated on multi-digit arithmetic performance. Chinese adults made more errors that reflect verbal-production processes that may occur after retrieval, whereas Canadian adults made more e...

Collaboration


Dive into the Jo-Anne LeFevre's collaboration.

Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Researchain Logo
Decentralizing Knowledge