Marcie Penner-Wilger
Carleton University
Network
Latest external collaboration on country level. Dive into details by clicking on the dots.
Publication
Featured researches published by Marcie Penner-Wilger.
Child Development | 2010
Jo-Anne LeFevre; Lisa Fast; Sheri-Lynn Skwarchuk; Brenda Smith-Chant; Jeffrey Bisanz; Deepthi Kamawar; Marcie Penner-Wilger
A model of the relations among cognitive precursors, early numeracy skill, and mathematical outcomes was tested for 182 children from 4.5 to 7.5 years of age. The model integrates research from neuroimaging, clinical populations, and normal development in children and adults. It includes 3 precursor pathways: quantitative, linguistic, and spatial attention. These pathways (a) contributed independently to early numeracy skills during preschool and kindergarten and (b) related differentially to performance on a variety of mathematical outcomes 2 years later. The success of the model in accounting for performance highlights the need to understand the fundamental underlying skills that contribute to diverse forms of mathematical competence.
Memory & Cognition | 2002
Marcie Penner-Wilger; Craig Leth-Steensen; Jo-Anne LeFevre
Is the locus of the problem-size effect in mental arithmetic different across cultures? In a novel approach to this question, the ex-Gaussian distributional model was applied to response times for large (e.g., 8 × 9) and small (e.g., 2 × 3) problems obtained from Chinese and Canadian graduate students in a multiplication production task (LeFevre & Liu, 1997). The problem-size effect for the Chinese group occurred in ώ (the mean of the normal component), whereas the problem-size effect for the Canadian group occurred in both ώ and ℸ (the mean of the exponential component). The results support the position that the problem-size effect for the Chinese group is purely a memory-retrieval effect, whereas for the Canadian group, it is an effect of both retrieval and the use of nonretrieval solution procedures.
Journal of Experimental Child Psychology | 2010
Deepthi Kamawar; Jo-Anne LeFevre; Jeffrey Bisanz; Lisa Fast; Sheri-Lynn Skwarchuk; Brenda Smith-Chant; Marcie Penner-Wilger
Most children who are older than 6 years of age apply essential counting principles when they enumerate a set of objects. Essential principles include (a) one-to-one correspondence between items and count words, (b) stable order of the count words, and (c) cardinality-that the last number refers to numerosity. We found that the acquisition of a fourth principle, that the order in which items are counted is irrelevant, follows a different trajectory. The majority of 5- to 11-year-olds indicated that the order in which objects were counted was relevant, favoring a left-to-right, top-to-bottom order of counting. Only some 10- and 11-year-olds applied the principle of order irrelevance, and this knowledge was unrelated to their numeration skill. We conclude that the order irrelevance principle might not play an important role in the development of childrens conceptual knowledge of counting.
Memory & Cognition | 2006
Jamie I. D. Campbell; Marcie Penner-Wilger
Does numeral format (e.g., 4 + 8 vs. four + eight) affect calculation per se? University students (N=47) solved single-digit addition problems presented as Arabic digits or English words and reported their strategies (memory retrieval or procedures such as counting or transformation). Decomposition of the response time (RT) distributions into μ (reflecting shift) and t (reflecting skew) confirmed that retrieval trials contributed predominantly to μ, whereas procedure trials contributed predominantly to τ. The format × problem size RT interaction (i.e., greater word-format RT costs for large problems than for small problems) was associated entirely with μ and not with τ. Reported use of procedures presented a corresponding format × size interaction. Together, these results indicate that, relative to the well-practiced digit format, the unfamiliar word format disrupts number-fact retrieval and promotes use of procedural strategies.
Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology | 2010
Runa Das; Jo-Anne LeFevre; Marcie Penner-Wilger
Are negative numbers processed differently from positive numbers in arithmetic problems? In two experiments, adults (N = 66) solved standard addition and subtraction problems such as 3 + 4 and 7 – 4 and recasted versions that included explicit negative signs—that is, 3 – (–4), 7 + (–4), and (–4) + 7. Solution times on the recasted problems were slower than those on standard problems, but the effect was much larger for addition than subtraction. The negative sign may prime subtraction in both kinds of recasted problem. Problem size effects were the same or smaller in recasted than in standard problems, suggesting that the recasted formats did not interfere with mental calculation. These results suggest that the underlying conceptual structure of the problem (i.e., addition vs. subtraction) is more important for solution processes than the presence of negative numbers.
Journal of Experimental Child Psychology | 2006
Jo-Anne LeFevre; Brenda Smith-Chant; Lisa Fast; Sheri-Lynn Skwarchuk; Erin Sargla; Jesse S. Arnup; Marcie Penner-Wilger; Jeffrey Bisanz; Deepthi Kamawar
Proceedings of the Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society | 2007
Marcie Penner-Wilger; Lisa Fast; Jo-Anne LaFevre; Brenda Smith-Chant; Sheri-Lynn Skwarchuck; Deepthi Kamawar; Jeffrey Bisanz
Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology | 2006
Jo-Anne LeFevre; Diana DeStefano; Marcie Penner-Wilger; Karen E. Daley
Archive | 2008
Marcie Penner-Wilger; Michael L. Anderson
Proceedings of the Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society | 2009
Jeffrey Bisnaz; Lisa Fast; Deepthi Kamawar; Jo-Anne LeFevre; Marcie Penner-Wilger; Sheri-Lynn Skwarchuk; Brenda Smith-Chant