Network


Latest external collaboration on country level. Dive into details by clicking on the dots.

Hotspot


Dive into the research topics where John Gibbon is active.

Publication


Featured researches published by John Gibbon.


Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences | 1984

Scalar Timing in Memory

John Gibbon; Russell M. Church; Warren H. Meck

A recent report of ours’ proposed an information-processing account of temporal generalization. The account posited a clock process, which was the basic time measurement device, and working and reference memory for storing the output of the clock either temporarily or relatively permanently. Records of time intervals in working and reference memory were then compared using a binary decision process, which dictated responding or not responding. The analysis concentrated on a relativistic Weber’s law property of the data from temporal generalization, and the constraints this property imposed on sources of variance in the information-processing stages. Our purpose here is to summarize that work and generalize the model in two ways: First we consider several sources of variance operating simultaneously. The original analysis demonstrated that if only one source of variance is present, it must be a scalar source, that is, it must result in a variable memory for which variance increases with the square of the mean.’ In the generalized account proposed here, we will develop the conclusion that scalar sources dominate in some time ranges, while other sources may dominate in others. These ideas are then applied to two additional timing tasks with different characteristics.


Psychological Review | 2000

Time, rate, and conditioning

C. R. Gallistel; John Gibbon

The authors draw together and develop previous timing models for a broad range of conditioning phenomena to reveal their common conceptual foundations: First, conditioning depends on the learning of the temporal intervals between events and the reciprocals of these intervals, the rates of event occurrence. Second, remembered intervals and rates translate into observed behavior through decision processes whose structure is adapted to noise in the decision variables. The noise and the uncertainties consequent on it have both subjective and objective origins. A third feature of these models is their timescale invariance, which the authors argue is a very important property evident in the available experimental data. This conceptual framework is similar to the psychophysical conceptual framework in which contemporary models of sensory processing are rooted. The authors contrast it with the associative conceptual framework.


Current Opinion in Neurobiology | 1997

Toward a neurobiology of temporal cognition: Advances and challenges.

John Gibbon; Chara Malapani; Corby L. Dale; C. R. Gallistel

A rich tradition of normative psychophysics has identified two ubiquitous properties of interval timing: the scalar property, a strong form of Webers law, and ratio comparison mechanisms. Finding the neural substrate of these properties is a major challenge for neurobiology. Recently, advances have been made in our understanding of the brain structures important for timing, especially the basal ganglia and the cerebellum. Surgical intervention or diseases of the cerebellum generally result in increased variability in temporal processing, whereas both clock and memory effects are seen for neurotransmitter interventions, lesions and diseases of the basal ganglia. We propose that cerebellar dysfunction may induce deregulation of tonic thalamic tuning, which disrupts gating of the mnemonic temporal information generated in the basal ganglia through striato-thalamo-cortical loops.


Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience | 1998

Coupled Temporal Memories in Parkinson's Disease: A Dopamine-Related Dysfunction

Chara Malapani; Brian C. Rakitin; Richard Levy; Warren H. Meck; Bernard Deweer; Bruno Dubois; John Gibbon

Dysfunction of the basal ganglia and the brain nuclei interconnected with them leads to disturbances of movement and cognition, including disordered timing of movement and perceptual timing deflcits. Patients with Parkinsons disease (PD) were studied in temporal reproduction tasks. We examined PD patients when brain dopamine (DA) transmission was impaired (OFF state) and when DA transmission was reestablished, at the time of maximal clinical beneflt following administration of levodopa + apomorphine (ON state). Patients reproduced target times of 8 and 21 sec trained in blocked trials with the peak interval procedure, which were veridical in the ON state, comparable to normative performance by healthy young and aged controls (Experiment 1). In the OFF state, temporal reproduction was impaired in both accuracy and precision (variance). The 8-sec signal was reproduced as longer and the 21-sec signal was reproduced as shorter than they actually were (Experiment 1). This fimigrationfl effect was dependent upon training of two different durations. When PD patients were trained on 21 sec only (Experiment 2), they showed a reproduction error in the long direction, opposite to the error produced under the dual training condition of Experiment 1. The results are discussed as a mutual attraction between temporal processing systems, in memory and clock stages, when dopaminergic regulation in the striatum is dysfunctional.


Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes | 1994

Application of scalar timing theory to individual trials.

Russell M. Church; Warren H. Meck; John Gibbon

Our purpose was to infer the characteristics of the internal clock, temporal memory, and decision processes involved in temporal generalization behavior on the basis of the analysis of individual trials. Three groups of 10 rats each were trained on a peak procedure with reinforcement at 15, 30, or 60 s, with several nonfood trial durations. On nonfood trials, the mean response rate gradually increased to a maximum near the time that reinforcement sometimes occurred and then gradually decreased. Individual trials were characterized by a period of high response rate, preceded and followed by a low response rate. The covariance pattern among measures of the temporal characteristics of the high response rate (start, stop, middle, and spread) supported a parallel, scalar timing model in which animals used on each trial a single sample from memory of the time of reinforcement and separate response thresholds to decide when to start and stop responding. An alternative model, the quasi-serial model (J. Gibbon & R. M. Church, 1992), was not consistent with the obtained relationships between covariances or with the scalar property seen across different nonfood signal durations.


Learning and Motivation | 1991

Origins of scalar timing

John Gibbon

I have attempted to trace here a personal and theoretical odyssey through, one might say, both chronological time over some years and subjective time over the auges in which we usually observe in an animal behavior


Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance | 2000

Differential effects of auditory and visual signals on clock speed and temporal memory.

Trevor B. Penney; John Gibbon; Warren H. Meck

The effects of signal modality on duration classification in college students were studied with the duration bisection task. When auditory and visual signals were presented in the same test session and shared common anchor durations, visual signals were classified as shorter than equivalent duration auditory signals. This occurred when auditory and visual signals were presented sequentially in the same test session and when presented simultaneously but asynchronously. Presentation of a single modality signal within a test session, or both modalities but with different anchor durations did not result in classification differences. The authors posit a model in which auditory and visual signals drive an internal clock at different rates. The clock rate difference is due to an attentional effect on the mode switch and is revealed only when the memories for the short and long anchor durations consist of a mix of contributions from accumulations generated by both the fast auditory and slower visual clock rates. When this occurs auditory signals seem longer than visual signals relative to the composite memory representation.


Learning and Motivation | 1991

Human bisection at the geometric mean

Lorraine G. Allan; John Gibbon

Abstract Theoretical developments have proceeded relatively independently in the human and animal timing literatures, even though many of the issues addressed have been similar. In the animal timing literature, an influential model is Scalar Timing. There have been few direct applications of Scalar Timing to human timing. The main purpose of the present paper is to present human timing data from experiments designed specifically to evaluate Scalar Timing. A human analog of the animal bisection discrimination procedure was used. The human bisection data were consonant with Scalar Timing: bisection was at the GM of the two referent durations and psychometric functions superposed when normalized by the bisection point.


Cognition | 1990

Representation of time

John Gibbon; Russell M. Church

Memory representation for time was studied in two settings. First, an analysis of timing in a laboratory analog of a foraging situation revealed that departure times from a patchy resource followed a Weber Law-like property implied by scalar timing. A trial-by-trial analysis was then pursued in a similar but more structured experimental paradigm, the Peak procedure. Study of covariance structures in the data implicated scalar variance in the memory for time as well as in the decision process, but the correlation pattern ruled out multiple access to memory within a trial.


Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes | 1998

Scalar expectancy theory and peak-interval timing in humans

Brian C. Rakitin; John Gibbon; Trevor B. Penney; Chara Malapani; Sean C. Hinton; Warren H. Meck

The properties of the internal clock, temporal memory, and decision processes used to time short durations were investigated. The peak-interval procedure was used to evaluate the timing of 8-, 12-, and 21-s intervals, and analyses were conducted on the mean response functions and on individual trials. A distractor task prevented counting, and visual feedback on accuracy and precision was provided after each trial. Mean response distributions were (a) centered at the appropriate real-time criteria, (b) highly symmetrical, and (c) scalar in their variability. Analysis of individual trials indicated more memory variability relative to response threshold variability. Taken together, these results demonstrate that humans show the same qualitative timing properties that other animals do, but with some quantitative differences.

Collaboration


Dive into the John Gibbon's collaboration.

Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Trevor B. Penney

National University of Singapore

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

C. M. Locurto

College of the Holy Cross

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Dani Brunner

University of Cambridge

View shared research outputs
Researchain Logo
Decentralizing Knowledge