Network


Latest external collaboration on country level. Dive into details by clicking on the dots.

Hotspot


Dive into the research topics where John Stein is active.

Publication


Featured researches published by John Stein.


Trends in Neurosciences | 1997

To see but not to read; the magnocellular theory of dyslexia

John Stein; Vincent Walsh

Developmental dyslexics often complain that small letters appear to blur and move around when they are trying to read. Anatomical, electrophysiological, psychophysical and brain-imaging studies have all contributed to elucidating the functional organization of these and other visual confusions. They emerge not from damage to a single visual relay but from abnormalities of the magnocellular component of the visual system, which is specialized for processing fast temporal information. The m-stream culminates in the posterior parietal cortex, which plays an important role in guiding visual attention. The evidence is consistent with an increasingly sophisticated account of dyslexia that does not single out either phonological, or visual or motor deficits. Rather, temporal processing in all three systems seems to be impaired. Dyslexics may be unable to process fast incoming sensory information adequately in any domain.


American Journal of Human Genetics | 1999

A Quantitative-Trait Locus on Chromosome 6p Influences Different Aspects of Developmental Dyslexia

Simon E. Fisher; Angela J. Marlow; J A Lamb; Elena Maestrini; Dianne F. Williams; Alex J. Richardson; Daniel E. Weeks; John Stein; Anthony P. Monaco

Recent application of nonparametric-linkage analysis to reading disability has implicated a putative quantitative-trait locus (QTL) on the short arm of chromosome 6. In the present study, we use QTL methods to evaluate linkage to the 6p25-21.3 region in a sample of 181 sib pairs from 82 nuclear families that were selected on the basis of a dyslexic proband. We have assessed linkage directly for several quantitative measures that should correlate with different components of the phenotype, rather than using a single composite measure or employing categorical definitions of subtypes. Our measures include the traditional IQ/reading discrepancy score, as well as tests of word recognition, irregular-word reading, and nonword reading. Pointwise analysis by means of sib-pair trait differences suggests the presence, in 6p21.3, of a QTL influencing multiple components of dyslexia, in particular the reading of irregular words (P=.0016) and nonwords (P=.0024). A complementary statistical approach involving estimation of variance components supports these findings (irregular words, P=.007; nonwords, P=.0004). Multipoint analyses place the QTL within the D6S422-D6S291 interval, with a peak around markers D6S276 and D6S105 consistently identified by approaches based on trait differences (irregular words, P=.00035; nonwords, P=.0035) and variance components (irregular words, P=.007; nonwords, P=.0038). Our findings indicate that the QTL affects both phonological and orthographic skills and is not specific to phoneme awareness, as has been previously suggested. Further studies will be necessary to obtain a more precise localization of this QTL, which may lead to the isolation of one of the genes involved in developmental dyslexia.


Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences | 1996

Auditory Temporal Coding in Dyslexia

Ken I. McAnally; John Stein

Developmental dyslexia is generally believed to result from impaired linguistic processing rather than from deficits in low-level sensory function. Challenging this view, we studied the perception of non-verbal acoustic stimuli and low-level auditory evoked potentials in dyslexic adults. Compared with matched controls, dyslexics were selectively impaired in tasks (frequency discrimination and binaural unmasking) which rely on decoding neural discharges phase-locked to the fine structure of the stimulus. Furthermore, this ability to use phase-locking was related to reading ability. In addition, the evoked potential reflecting phase-locked discharges was significantly smaller in dyslexics. These results demonstrate a low-level auditory impairment in dyslexia traceable to the brainstem nuclei.


The Lancet | 1998

Metabolic abnormalities in developmental dyslexia detected by 1H magnetic resonance spectroscopy

Caroline Rae; M A Lee; Ruth M Dixon; Andrew M. Blamire; Campbell H. Thompson; Peter Styles; Joel B. Talcott; Alexandra J. Richardson; John Stein

BACKGROUND Neurological and physiological deficits have been reported in the brain in developmental dyslexia. The temporoparietal cortex has been directly implicated in dyslexic dysfunction, and substantial indirect evidence suggests that the cerebellum is also implicated. We wanted to find out whether the neurological and physiological deficits manifested as biochemical changes in the brain. METHODS We obtained localised proton magnetic resonance spectra bilaterally from the temporo-parietal cortex and cerebellum of 14 well-defined dyslexic men and 15 control men of similar age. FINDINGS We found biochemical differences between dyslexic men and controls in the left temporo-parietal lobe (ratio of choline-containing compounds [Cho] to N-acetylaspartate [NA] p< or =0.01) and right cerebellum (Cho/NA, p< or = 0.01; creatine [Cre] to NA p< or =0.05; (not significant). We found lateral biochemical differences in dyslexic men in both these brain regions (Cho/NA in temporo-parietal lobe, left vs right, p< or =0.01; Cre/NA in cerebellum, left vs right, p< or =0.001). We found no such lateral differences in controls. There was no significant relation between the degree of contralateral chemical difference and handedness in dyslexic or control men. INTERPRETATION We suggest that the observed differences reflect changes in cell density in the temporo-parietal lobe in developmental dyslexia and that the altered cerebral structural symmetry in dyslexia is associated with abnormal development of cells or intracellular connections or both. The cerebellum is biochemically asymmetric in dyslexic men, indicating altered development of this organ. These differences provide direct evidence of the involvement of the cerebellum in dyslexic dysfunction.


Neuropsychologia | 2003

Visual motion sensitivity and reading.

John Stein

Reading is more difficult than speaking because an arbitrary set of visual symbols must be rapidly identified, ordered and translated into the sounds they represent. Many poor readers have particular problems with the rapid visual processing required for these tasks because they have a mild impairment of the visual magnocellular system. This deficit has been demonstrated using neuropathological, evoked potential, functional magnetic resonance imaging and psychophysical techniques. The sensitivity of the M-system in both good and bad readers correlates with their orthographic abilities, suggesting that the M-system plays an important part in their development. This role is probably to mediate steady direction of visual attention and eye fixations on words. Thus many children with reading difficulties have unsteady eye control and this causes the letters they are trying to read to appear to move around, so that they cannot tell what order they are meant to be in. Therefore, boosting M-performance using yellow filters, or training eye fixation, can improve reading performance very significantly. Several genetic linkage studies have associated reading difficulties with the MHC control region on the short arm of chromosome 6. This system has recently been shown to help regulate the differentiation of M-cells. This association could also explain the high incidence of autoimmune conditions in poor readers. Other chromosomal sites are associated with the metabolism of polyunsaturated fatty acids (PUFAs) as found in fish oils, and this could explain why PUFA supplements can improve reading.


Pain | 2000

Motor cortex stimulation for chronic neuropathic pain: a preliminary study of 10 cases.

Dawn Carroll; Carole Joint; Nikki Maartens; David Shlugman; John Stein; Tipu Z. Aziz

Abstract There is growing evidence to support the use of motor cortex stimulation (MCS) in the management of patients with chronic neuropathic pain. A prospective audit of ten patients using a modified staged technique for motor cortex implantation provides further evidence for the analgesic effectiveness of this technique. Ten patients suffering from phantom limb pain (n=3), post stroke pain (n=5), post traumatic neuralgia secondary to gunshot injury to the brain stem (n=1) and brachyalgia secondary to neuro‐fibromatosis (n=1) were treated between November 1995 and February 1998. All ten patients had failed to respond to previous multiple pain therapies. Patients were evaluated pre and post‐operatively by an independent pain specialist. The overall response rate was 50%, with 5/10 patients reporting short term relief (>50% pain relief) and long‐term benefit in 4/5 of patients who initially responded to intermittent cortical stimulation (longest follow up 31 months after implantation). Of those patients who benefited two had post stroke pain, two phantom limb pain and one post‐traumatic neuralgia. We conclude that motor cortex stimulation is an effective analgesic intervention in some patients with chronic neuropathic pain, but it is difficult if not impossible to predict those patients who may respond to treatment prior to implantation. Randomised controlled trials are now urgently needed to test the effectiveness of motor cortex stimulation under double‐blind conditions.


Neuroreport | 2001

Are dyslexics' visual deficits limited to measures of dorsal stream function?

Peter C. Hansen; John Stein; Sam R. Orde; Jonathan L. Winter; Joel B. Talcott

We tested the hypothesis that the differences in performance between developmental dyslexics and controls on visual tasks are specific for the detection of dynamic stimuli. We found that dyslexics were less sensitive than controls to coherent motion in dynamic random dot displays. However, their sensitivity to control measures of static visual form coherence was not significantly different from that of controls. This dissociation of dyslexics’ performance on measures that are suggested to tap the sensitivity of different extrastriate visual areas provides evidence for an impairment specific to the detection of dynamic properties of global stimuli, perhaps resulting from selective deficits in dorsal stream functions.


Developmental Medicine & Child Neurology | 2001

Lateralized cognitive deficits in children following cerebellar lesions

Richard B Scott; Catherine J. Stoodley; Philip Anslow; Caroline Paul; John Stein; Elaine Sugden; Chris Mitchell

The aim of this preliminary study was to examine the developing cognitive profiles of children with cerebellar tumours in a consecutive series of clinical patients. MRI and longitudinal intellectual profiles were obtained on seven children (two females, five males; mean age 3 years at diagnosis; mean age 7 years at first assessment). Tumours in three of the children were astrocytomas; of the remaining tumours, two were medulloblastomas, one low-grade glioma, and one ependymoma. In right-handed children, we observed an association between greater damage to right cerebellar structures and a plateauing in verbal and/or literacy skills. In contrast, greater damage to left cerebellar structures was associated with delayed or impaired non-verbal/spatial skills. Long-term cognitive development of the children studied tentatively supports a role for the cerebellum in learning/development. These findings suggest that lateralized cerebellar damage may selectively impair the development of cognitive functions subserved by the contralateral cerebral hemisphere and, in addition, that all children with cerebellar lesions in early childhood should routinely undergo long-term monitoring of their intellectual development.


Journal of Learning Disabilities | 1995

Verbal and Visual Problems in Reading Disability

Guinevere F. Eden; John Stein; Maria H. Wood; Frank B. Wood

Most individuals interested in reading disability favor the view that disordered language processing is the main cause of childrens reading problems and that visual problems are seldom, if ever, responsible. Nevertheless, in a preliminary study (Eden, Stein, & Wood, 1993) we showed that visuospatial and oculomotor tests can be used to differentiate children with reading disabilities from nondisabled children. In the present study we investigated a larger sample of children to see if these findings held true. Using 93 children from the Bowman Gray Learning Disability Project (mean age = 11.3 years; 54 boys, 39 girls), we compared the phonological and visuospatial abilities of nondisabled children (children whose reading at fifth grade rated a Woodcock-Johnson reading standardized score between 85 and 115), and children with reading disability (whose reading standardized score was below 85 on the Woodcock-Johnson). In addition to performing poorly on verbal tests, the children with reading disability were significantly worse than nondisabled children at many visual and eye-movement tasks. A high proportion of the variance (68%) in reading ability of both the nondisabled children and those with reading disability could be predicted by combining visual and phonological scores in a multiple regression. These results provide further support for the hypothesis that reading disability may, to some extent, result from dysfunction of the visual and oculomotor systems.


Journal of Medical Genetics | 2004

Putative functional alleles of DYX1C1 are not associated with dyslexia susceptibility in a large sample of sibling pairs from the UK

Thomas S. Scerri; Simon E. Fisher; Clyde Francks; Il MacPhie; Silvia Paracchini; Alex J. Richardson; John Stein; Anthony P. Monaco

Developmental dyslexia is diagnosed as a specific impairment in reading ability, despite adequate intelligence and educational opportunity,1 that affects approximately 5% of schoolchildren.2 Much evidence has been accumulated from twin and family based studies to indicate that dyslexia can have a hereditary basis, but that the genetic aetiology is complex, involving multiple risk factors.1–3 Linkage analysis has identified numerous genomic regions that may harbour susceptibility genes influencing dyslexia, including on chromosomes 1, 2, 3, 6, 15, and 18, with varying degrees of reproducibility.1,2 The first of these linkages was reported two decades ago,4 to the centromere of chromosome 15. Although subsequent studies failed to replicate linkage to this specific region,5 there is evidence for linkage elsewhere on chromosome 15, particularly at 15q21 ( DYX1 , OMIM 127700).6,7 Recently, DYX1C1 (also known as EKN1 ) was proposed as the gene underlying the putative effect on 15q21.8 This was initially based on studies of a balanced translocation, t(2;15)(q11;q21), co-segregating with reading problems within a single nuclear family from Finland.9 The 15q21 breakpoint in this family directly disrupts DYX1C1 , in an interval that includes exons 8 and 9 (fig 1). Investigation of DYX1C1 in individuals from 20 additional Finnish families with multiple cases of dyslexia led to the identification of eight single nucleotide polymorphisms (SNPs). Two of these SNPs were found to associate with dyslexia in these families, and in additional Finnish affected cases and controls. It was proposed that these two associated SNPs altered the expression or function of DYX1C1 , one by altering a transcription factor binding site, the other as a result of a premature truncation of the protein product by four amino acids, thereby leading to increased risk of developing dyslexia.8 Figure 1  Location of DYX1C1 on chromosome 15. Top: …

Collaboration


Dive into the John Stein's collaboration.

Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Sue Fowler

Royal Berkshire Hospital

View shared research outputs
Researchain Logo
Decentralizing Knowledge