Network


Latest external collaboration on country level. Dive into details by clicking on the dots.

Hotspot


Dive into the research topics where Judith Koppehele-Gossel is active.

Publication


Featured researches published by Judith Koppehele-Gossel.


Nature Neuroscience | 2014

Induction of self awareness in dreams through frontal low current stimulation of gamma activity

Ursula Voss; Romain Holzmann; Allan Hobson; Walter Paulus; Judith Koppehele-Gossel; Ansgar Klimke; Michael A. Nitsche

Recent findings link fronto-temporal gamma electroencephalographic (EEG) activity to conscious awareness in dreams, but a causal relationship has not yet been established. We found that current stimulation in the lower gamma band during REM sleep influences ongoing brain activity and induces self-reflective awareness in dreams. Other stimulation frequencies were not effective, suggesting that higher order consciousness is indeed related to synchronous oscillations around 25 and 40 Hz.


Journal of Sleep Research | 2012

Lucid dreaming: an age-dependent brain dissociation

Ursula Voss; Clemens Frenzel; Judith Koppehele-Gossel; Allan Hobson

The current study focused on the distribution of lucid dreams in school children and young adults. The survey was conducted on a large sample of students aged 6–19 years. Questions distinguished between past and current experience with lucid dreams. Results suggest that lucid dreaming is quite pronounced in young children, its incidence rate drops at about age 16 years. Increased lucidity was found in those attending higher level compared with lower level schools. Taking methodological issues into account, we feel confident to propose a link between the natural occurrence of lucid dreaming and brain maturation.


Social Neuroscience | 2015

Weak encoding of faces predicts socially influenced judgments of facial attractiveness

Robert Schnuerch; Judith Koppehele-Gossel; Henning Gibbons

Conforming to the majority can be seen as a heuristic type of judgment, as it allows the individual to easily choose the most accurate or most socially acceptable type of behavior. People who process the currently to-be-judged items in a superficial, heuristic way should tend to conform to group judgment more than people processing these items in a systematic and elaborate way. We investigated this hypothesis using electroencephalography (EEG), analyzing whether the strength of neural encoding of faces was related to the tendency to adopt a group’s evaluative judgments regarding these faces. As expected, we found that the amplitude of the N170, a specific neural correlate of face encoding, was inversely related to conformity across participants: The weaker the faces were encoded, the more the majority response regarding the faces’ attractiveness was adopted instead of relying on the actual qualities of the faces. Applying neurophysiological methodology, we thus provide support for previous claims, based on behavioral data and theorizing, that social conformity is a heuristic type of judgment. We propose that weak encoding of judgment-relevant information is a typical, possibly even necessary, precursor of socially adjusted judgments, irrespective of one’s current motivational goal (i.e., to be accurate or accepted).


Brain and Language | 2016

A brain electrical signature of left-lateralized semantic activation from single words☆

Judith Koppehele-Gossel; Robert Schnuerch; Henning Gibbons

Lesion and imaging studies consistently indicate a left-lateralization of semantic language processing in human temporo-parietal cortex. Surprisingly, electrocortical measures, which allow a direct assessment of brain activity and the tracking of cognitive functions with millisecond precision, have not yet been used to capture this hemispheric lateralization, at least with respect to posterior portions of this effect. Using event-related potentials, we employed a simple single-word reading paradigm to compare neural activity during three tasks requiring different degrees of semantic processing. As expected, we were able to derive a simple temporo-parietal left-right asymmetry index peaking around 300ms into word processing that neatly tracks the degree of semantic activation. The validity of this measure in specifically capturing verbal semantic activation was further supported by a significant relation to verbal intelligence. We thus posit that it represents a promising tool to monitor verbal semantic processing in the brain with little technological effort and in a minimal experimental setup.


Brain and Cognition | 2018

The posterior semantic asymmetry (PSA): An early brain electrical signature of semantic activation from written words

Judith Koppehele-Gossel; Robert Schnuerch; Henning Gibbons

HIGHLIGHTSReplication of a left‐lateralized semantic processing negativity.Left‐temporal origin suggested by CSD analysis.Validity indicated by correlations to measures of verbal intelligence.Component reflects the effort of concept activation from verbal material. ABSTRACT This study replicates and extends the findings of Koppehele‐Gossel, Schnuerch, and Gibbons (2016) of a posterior semantic asymmetry (PSA) in event‐related brain potentials (ERPs), which closely tracks the time course and degree of semantic activation from single visual words. This negativity peaked 300ms after word onset, was derived by subtracting right‐ from left‐side activity, and was larger in a semantic task compared to two non‐semantic control tasks. The validity of the PSA in reflecting the effort to activate word meaning was again attested by a negative correlation between the meaning‐specific PSA increase and verbal intelligence, even after controlling for nonverbal intelligence. Extending prior work, current source density (CSD) transformation was used. CSD results were consistent with a left temporo‐parietal cortical origin of the PSA. Moreover, no PSA was found for pictorial material, suggesting that the component reflects early semantic processing specific to verbal stimuli.


Psychophysiology | 2018

Affective priming and cognitive load: Event-related potentials suggest an interplay of implicit affect misattribution and strategic inhibition

Henning Gibbons; Laura-Effi Seib-Pfeifer; Judith Koppehele-Gossel; Robert Schnuerch

Prior research suggests that the affective priming effect denoting prime-congruent evaluative judgments about neutral targets preceded by affective primes increases when the primes are processed less deeply. This has been taken as evidence for greater affect misattribution. However, no study so far has combined an experimental manipulation of the depth of prime processing with the benefits of ERPs. Forty-seven participants made like/dislike responses about Korean ideographs following 800-ms affective prime words while 64-channel EEG was recorded. In a randomized within-subject design, three levels of working-memory load were applied specifically during prime processing. Affective priming was significant for all loads and even tended to decrease over loads, although efficiency of the load manipulation was confirmed by reduced amplitudes of posterior attention-sensitive prime ERPs. Moreover, ERPs revealed greater explicit affective discrimination of the prime words as load increased, with strongest valence effects on central/centroparietal N400 and on the parietal/parietooccipital late positive complex under high load. This suggests that (a) participants by default tried to inhibit the processing of the primes affect, and (b) inhibition more often failed under cognitive load, thus causing emotional breakthrough that resulted in a binding of affect to the prime and, hence, reduced affect misattribution to the target. As a correlate of affective priming in the target ERP, medial-frontal negativity, a well-established marker of (low) stimulus value, increased with increasing negative affect of the prime. Findings support implicit prime-target affect transfer as a major source of affective priming, but also point to the role of strategic top-down processes.


Consciousness and Cognition | 2016

A template model of embodiment while dreaming: Proposal of a mini-me.

Judith Koppehele-Gossel; Ansgar Klimke; Karin Schermelleh-Engel; Ursula Voss

Dreams are usually centered around a dream self capable of tasks generally impossible in waking, e.g. flying or walking through walls. Moreover, the bodily dream self appears relatively stable and insensitive to changes of the embodied wake self, raising the question of whether and to what extent the dream self is embodied. To further explore its determinants, we tested whether the dream self would be affected by either pre-sleep focused attention to a body part or by its experimental alteration during the day. Choosing a repeated-measures design, we analyzed how often key words reflecting the experimental manipulations appeared in the dream reports. Results suggest that the dream self is not affected by these manipulations, strengthening the hypothesis that, in the majority of dreams, the dream self is only weakly embodied, utilizing a standard template of embodiment akin to a prototype of self operating independently from the physical waking self.


Aggression and Violent Behavior | 2013

Pro-criminal attitudes, intervention, and recidivism ☆

Rainer Banse; Judith Koppehele-Gossel; Lisa M. Kistemaker; Verena A. Werner; A. Schmidt


Law and Human Behavior | 2016

Validity of Content-Based Techniques to Distinguish True and Fabricated Statements: A Meta-Analysis

Verena A. Oberlader; Christoph Naefgen; Judith Koppehele-Gossel; Laura Katharina Josefine Lucia Quinten; Rainer Banse; A. Schmidt


Psychophysiology | 2016

Multiple neural signatures of social proof and deviance during the observation of other people's preferences

Robert Schnuerch; Jasmin Richter; Judith Koppehele-Gossel; Henning Gibbons

Collaboration


Dive into the Judith Koppehele-Gossel's collaboration.

Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Ursula Voss

Goethe University Frankfurt

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Ansgar Klimke

University of Düsseldorf

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

A. Schmidt

University of Luxembourg

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Researchain Logo
Decentralizing Knowledge