Kimberly Brenneman
Rutgers University
Network
Latest external collaboration on country level. Dive into details by clicking on the dots.
Publication
Featured researches published by Kimberly Brenneman.
Cognitive Development | 1996
Kimberly Brenneman; Christine Massey; Steven F. Machado; Rochel Gelman
Abstract Forty-eight children (mean age= 64.4 months, range = 52–75 months), unschooled in writing, were asked to draw a picture of and write the name for common objects depicted in line drawings. Analyses of the childrens videotaped action sequences while drawing and writing revealed reliable, systematic differences. For example, drawings were often made with continuous outlines that could be filled in, and marks were put on the page in a random fashion. “Writing” was characterized by discrete marks on the page arranged in a linear fashion and generated from left to right. We propose that young childrens plans for drawing and writing are constrained by domain-specific knowledge about words and objects. It follows that they have implicit knowledge of the fact that different notation systems must honor structural differences between the domains being notated.
Early Education and Development | 2011
Irena Nayfeld; Kimberly Brenneman; Rochel Gelman
Research Findings: This paper reports on childrens use of science materials in preschool classrooms during their free choice time. Baseline observations showed that children and teachers rarely spend time in the designated science area. An intervention was designed to “market” the science center by introducing children to 1 science tool, the balance scale. Baseline measures showed that children did not know the scales name or function. The intervention was expected to increase childrens use of the science area and their knowledge about the scale. Childrens voluntary presence and exploration in the science area increased after the balance scale intervention compared to in comparison classrooms. Furthermore, children who participated in this intervention demonstrated improved knowledge about the scales function, whereas students in the comparison group did not. Practice or Policy: Adults can increase childrens autonomous exploration of science tools and materials, and their knowledge about them, by offering particular kinds of large-group learning experiences.
Early Education and Development | 2016
Peter J. Marshall; Kimberly Brenneman
The purpose of this special issue of Early Education & Development is to showcase research on young children’s understanding of the living world. In this Introduction our aim is to tie together the various articles in the issue by identifying key themes that run through multiple articles. We also provide some reflections on implications of the studies for policy and practice, including prescriptions for future work on the integrative study of children’s biological understanding in the context of the early education classroom. One important theme in the study of children’s biological understanding is folkbiology, which refers to the theoretical frameworks of beliefs and expectations about living things that emerge in the absence of formal instruction (Solomon & Zaitchik, 2012). There is increasing interest in the factors that shape the development of folkbiological knowledge (Gelman, 2009; Medin, Waxman, Woodring, & Washinawatok, 2010), a theme that is apparent throughout the special issue. The most prominent example is the article by Taverna, Medin, and Waxman (2016), who provide new insights from their work with the Wichi, an indigenous group from a remote forest region in Argentina. This study documents how Wichi children organize the natural world into categories based on taxonomic or perceptual similarities, on ecological factors such as shared habitat, and on beliefs about animacy and spiritual status. The emergence of these categories guides a view of the natural world that is distinctly less anthropocentric (i.e., less human centered) than the view adopted by children in many Western societies, especially urban ones. The theme of anthropocentrism is also apparent in the article by Ruckert (2016), who examines the intersection of folkbiological thinking with the development of moral reasoning about environmental concerns. Working in the Pacific Northwest of the United States, Ruckert examines children’s responses to questions about values, obligations, and rights concerning endangered species. She contrasts anthropocentric thinking with a biocentric form of moral reasoning in which the natural world has moral standing independent of its value to humans. Ruckert finds that between 7 and 10 years of age, children’s thinking shifts from human-centered concerns to more biocentric reasoning. Continuing the theme of influences on children’s thinking about the natural world, the review by Longbottom and Slaughter (2016) examines how type of upbringing (urban vs. rural) and pet ownership affect children’s reasoning about the relation of humans to other animals, the connections between different species, and basic knowledge about animal biology. After showing how differences in children’s biological understandings are often associated with their direct experiences with outdoor experiences and with animals, the authors suggest that intervention research is needed on underlying mechanisms. Related anecdotal evidence that outdoor experiences influence children’s biological understanding comes from the article by Tao (2016). In a Chinese sample of children ages 4 to 6 years, children who consistently applied a biological justification for plants as living things often mentioned their hands-on experience and involvement with growing flowers and vegetables with their parents or grandparents.
Early Childhood Research Quarterly | 2004
Rochel Gelman; Kimberly Brenneman
Archive | 1994
Rochel Gelman; Kimberly Brenneman
Early Childhood Education Journal | 2008
Kimberly Brenneman; Ines F. Louro
Brookes Publishing Company | 2009
Rochel Gelman; Kimberly Brenneman; Moises Roman
Archive | 2012
Rochel Gelman; Kimberly Brenneman
Early Childhood Education Journal | 2018
Kimberly Brenneman; Alissa Lange; Irena Nayfeld
Proceedings of the Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society | 2008
Gregg Solomon; Rochel Gelman; Doug Medin; Nancy Nersessian; Laura Schulz; Megan Bang; Christine Massey; Kimberly Brenneman; Wendy Newsetter