Network


Latest external collaboration on country level. Dive into details by clicking on the dots.

Hotspot


Dive into the research topics where Kimberly Brenneman is active.

Publication


Featured researches published by Kimberly Brenneman.


Cognitive Development | 1996

Young Children's Plans Differ for Writing and Drawing.

Kimberly Brenneman; Christine Massey; Steven F. Machado; Rochel Gelman

Abstract Forty-eight children (mean age= 64.4 months, range = 52–75 months), unschooled in writing, were asked to draw a picture of and write the name for common objects depicted in line drawings. Analyses of the childrens videotaped action sequences while drawing and writing revealed reliable, systematic differences. For example, drawings were often made with continuous outlines that could be filled in, and marks were put on the page in a random fashion. “Writing” was characterized by discrete marks on the page arranged in a linear fashion and generated from left to right. We propose that young childrens plans for drawing and writing are constrained by domain-specific knowledge about words and objects. It follows that they have implicit knowledge of the fact that different notation systems must honor structural differences between the domains being notated.


Early Education and Development | 2011

Science in the Classroom: Finding a Balance Between Autonomous Exploration and Teacher-Led Instruction in Preschool Settings

Irena Nayfeld; Kimberly Brenneman; Rochel Gelman

Research Findings: This paper reports on childrens use of science materials in preschool classrooms during their free choice time. Baseline observations showed that children and teachers rarely spend time in the designated science area. An intervention was designed to “market” the science center by introducing children to 1 science tool, the balance scale. Baseline measures showed that children did not know the scales name or function. The intervention was expected to increase childrens use of the science area and their knowledge about the scale. Childrens voluntary presence and exploration in the science area increased after the balance scale intervention compared to in comparison classrooms. Furthermore, children who participated in this intervention demonstrated improved knowledge about the scales function, whereas students in the comparison group did not. Practice or Policy: Adults can increase childrens autonomous exploration of science tools and materials, and their knowledge about them, by offering particular kinds of large-group learning experiences.


Early Education and Development | 2016

Young Children’s Developing Understanding of the Biological World

Peter J. Marshall; Kimberly Brenneman

The purpose of this special issue of Early Education & Development is to showcase research on young children’s understanding of the living world. In this Introduction our aim is to tie together the various articles in the issue by identifying key themes that run through multiple articles. We also provide some reflections on implications of the studies for policy and practice, including prescriptions for future work on the integrative study of children’s biological understanding in the context of the early education classroom. One important theme in the study of children’s biological understanding is folkbiology, which refers to the theoretical frameworks of beliefs and expectations about living things that emerge in the absence of formal instruction (Solomon & Zaitchik, 2012). There is increasing interest in the factors that shape the development of folkbiological knowledge (Gelman, 2009; Medin, Waxman, Woodring, & Washinawatok, 2010), a theme that is apparent throughout the special issue. The most prominent example is the article by Taverna, Medin, and Waxman (2016), who provide new insights from their work with the Wichi, an indigenous group from a remote forest region in Argentina. This study documents how Wichi children organize the natural world into categories based on taxonomic or perceptual similarities, on ecological factors such as shared habitat, and on beliefs about animacy and spiritual status. The emergence of these categories guides a view of the natural world that is distinctly less anthropocentric (i.e., less human centered) than the view adopted by children in many Western societies, especially urban ones. The theme of anthropocentrism is also apparent in the article by Ruckert (2016), who examines the intersection of folkbiological thinking with the development of moral reasoning about environmental concerns. Working in the Pacific Northwest of the United States, Ruckert examines children’s responses to questions about values, obligations, and rights concerning endangered species. She contrasts anthropocentric thinking with a biocentric form of moral reasoning in which the natural world has moral standing independent of its value to humans. Ruckert finds that between 7 and 10 years of age, children’s thinking shifts from human-centered concerns to more biocentric reasoning. Continuing the theme of influences on children’s thinking about the natural world, the review by Longbottom and Slaughter (2016) examines how type of upbringing (urban vs. rural) and pet ownership affect children’s reasoning about the relation of humans to other animals, the connections between different species, and basic knowledge about animal biology. After showing how differences in children’s biological understandings are often associated with their direct experiences with outdoor experiences and with animals, the authors suggest that intervention research is needed on underlying mechanisms. Related anecdotal evidence that outdoor experiences influence children’s biological understanding comes from the article by Tao (2016). In a Chinese sample of children ages 4 to 6 years, children who consistently applied a biological justification for plants as living things often mentioned their hands-on experience and involvement with growing flowers and vegetables with their parents or grandparents.


Early Childhood Research Quarterly | 2004

Science learning pathways for young children

Rochel Gelman; Kimberly Brenneman


Archive | 1994

Mapping the mind: First principles can support both universal and culture-specific learning about number and music

Rochel Gelman; Kimberly Brenneman


Early Childhood Education Journal | 2008

Science Journals in the Preschool Classroom

Kimberly Brenneman; Ines F. Louro


Brookes Publishing Company | 2009

Preschool Pathways to Science (PrePS[TM]): Facilitating Scientific Ways of Thinking, Talking, Doing, and Understanding.

Rochel Gelman; Kimberly Brenneman; Moises Roman


Archive | 2012

Moving young “scientists-in-waiting” onto science learning pathways: Focus on observation.

Rochel Gelman; Kimberly Brenneman


Early Childhood Education Journal | 2018

Integrating STEM into Preschool Education; Designing a Professional Development Model in Diverse Settings

Kimberly Brenneman; Alissa Lange; Irena Nayfeld


Proceedings of the Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society | 2008

Cognitive Science and Education Research: Engaging Issues of Social Context - eScholarship

Gregg Solomon; Rochel Gelman; Doug Medin; Nancy Nersessian; Laura Schulz; Megan Bang; Christine Massey; Kimberly Brenneman; Wendy Newsetter

Collaboration


Dive into the Kimberly Brenneman's collaboration.

Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Laura Schulz

Massachusetts Institute of Technology

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Alissa Lange

East Tennessee State University

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Researchain Logo
Decentralizing Knowledge