Network


Latest external collaboration on country level. Dive into details by clicking on the dots.

Hotspot


Dive into the research topics where Laura M. Betancourt is active.

Publication


Featured researches published by Laura M. Betancourt.


Brain Research | 2006

Childhood poverty: specific associations with neurocognitive development.

Martha J. Farah; David Shera; Jessica H. Savage; Laura M. Betancourt; Joan M. Giannetta; Nancy L. Brodsky; Elsa Malmud; Hallam Hurt

Growing up in poverty is associated with reduced cognitive achievement as measured by standardized intelligence tests, but little is known about the underlying neurocognitive systems responsible for this effect. We administered a battery of tasks designed to tax-specific neurocognitive systems to healthy low and middle SES children screened for medical history and matched for age, gender and ethnicity. Higher SES was associated with better performance on the tasks, as expected, but the SES disparity was significantly nonuniform across neurocognitive systems. Pronounced differences were found in Left perisylvian/Language and Medial temporal/Memory systems, along with significant differences in Lateral/Prefrontal/Working memory and Anterior cingulate/Cognitive control and smaller, nonsignificant differences in Occipitotemporal/Pattern vision and Parietal/Spatial cognition.


Neuropsychologia | 2009

Executive cognitive functions and impulsivity as correlates of risk taking and problem behavior in preadolescents

Daniel Romer; Laura M. Betancourt; Joan M. Giannetta; Nancy L. Brodsky; Martha J. Farah; Hallam Hurt

Initiation of drug use and other risky behavior in preadolescence is associated with poor developmental outcomes. In this research, we examine models that ascribe the trajectory to (a) weak executive cognitive function (ECF), (b) early manifestation of externalizing problems, or (c) heightened levels of trait impulsivity. We test the explanatory power of these factors in a structural equation model with a community sample of 387 preadolescents ages 10-12 years. Participants were tested with a computerized battery of tasks to assess three facets of ECF (working memory, cognitive control, and reward processing) as well as with an audio assisted computerized self-interview to obtain reports of impulsivity and risk behaviors (use of cigarettes and alcohol as well as engaging in fighting and gambling for money) and a self-administered questionnaire to assess externalizing and internalizing problems. The best fitting model explained both early risk taking and externalizing symptoms as the result of individual differences in impulsivity. Although no ECF was directly related to risk taking, working memory and one measure of reward processing performance (reversal learning) were inversely related to impulsivity. The results are discussed in regard to theories of early risk taking with particular focus on the potential relation between ECF and impulsive behavior tendencies and the implications for early intervention to prevent the dysfunctional trajectory associated with early risk behavior.


NeuroImage | 2010

Early Parental Care Is Important for Hippocampal Maturation: Evidence from Brain Morphology in Humans

Hengyi Rao; Laura M. Betancourt; Joan M. Giannetta; Nancy L. Brodsky; Marc Korczykowski; Brian B. Avants; James C. Gee; Jiongjiong Wang; Hallam Hurt; John A. Detre; Martha J. Farah

The effects of early life experience on later brain structure and function have been studied extensively in animals, yet the relationship between childhood experience and normal brain development in humans remains largely unknown. Using a unique longitudinal data set including ecologically valid in-home measures of early experience during childhood (at age 4 and 8 years) and high-resolution structural brain imaging during adolescence (mean age 14 years), we examined the effects on later brain morphology of two dimensions of early experience: parental nurturance and environmental stimulation. Parental nurturance at age 4 predicts the volume of the left hippocampus in adolescence, with better nurturance associated with smaller hippocampal volume. In contrast, environmental stimulation did not correlate with hippocampal volume. Moreover, the association between hippocampal volume and parental nurturance disappears at age 8, supporting the existence of a sensitive developmental period for brain maturation. These findings indicate that variation in normal childhood experience is associated with differences in brain morphology, and hippocampal volume is specifically associated with early parental nurturance. Our results provide neuroimaging evidence supporting the important role of warm parental care during early childhood for brain maturation.


Developmental Science | 2011

Does adolescent risk taking imply weak executive function? A prospective study of relations between working memory performance, impulsivity, and risk taking in early adolescence.

Daniel Romer; Laura M. Betancourt; Nancy L. Brodsky; Joan M. Giannetta; Wang Yang; Hallam Hurt

Studies of brain development suggest that the increase in risk taking observed during adolescence may be due to insufficient prefrontal executive function compared to a more rapidly developing subcortical motivation system. We examined executive function as assessed by working memory ability in a community sample of youth (n = 387, ages 10 to 12 at baseline) in three annual assessments to determine its relation to two forms of impulsivity (sensation seeking and acting without thinking) and a wide range of risk and externalizing behavior. Using structural equation modeling, we tested a model in which differential activation of the dorsal and ventral striatum produces imbalance in the function of these brain regions. For youth high in sensation seeking, both regions were predicted to develop with age. However, for youth high in the tendency to act without thinking, the ventral striatum was expected to dominate. The model predicted that working memory ability would exhibit (1) early weakness in youth high in acting without thinking but (2) growing strength in those high in sensation seeking. In addition, it predicted that (3) acting without thinking would be more strongly related to risk and externalizing behavior than sensation seeking. Finally, it predicted that (4) controlling for acting without thinking, sensation seeking would predict later increases in risky and externalizing behavior. All four of these predictions were confirmed. The results indicate that the rise in sensation seeking that occurs during adolescence is not accompanied by a deficit in executive function and therefore requires different intervention strategies from those for youth whose impulsivity is characterized by early signs of acting without thinking.


Addiction | 2013

Working memory ability predicts trajectories of early alcohol use in adolescents: the mediational role of impulsivity

Atika Khurana; Daniel Romer; Laura M. Betancourt; Nancy L. Brodsky; Joan M. Giannetta; Hallam Hurt

AIMS (i) To evaluate the role of pre-existing weakness in working memory ability (WM) as a risk factor for early alcohol use as mediated by different forms of impulsivity and (ii) to assess the adverse effects of progressive alcohol use on variations in WM over time. DESIGN, SETTING AND PARTICIPANTS A community sample of 358 adolescents [48% males, mean(age) (baseline) = 11.4 ± 0.87 years] from a longitudinal cohort design, assessed annually over 4 consecutive years with less than 6% attrition. MEASUREMENTS Repeated assessments were conducted for the following key variables: WM (based on performance on four separate tasks), frequency of alcohol use (AU) and three forms of impulsivity, namely sensation seeking (SS), acting without thinking (AWT) and delay discounting (DD). Latent growth curve modeling procedures were used to identify individual trajectories of change for all key variables. FINDINGS Weakness in WM (at baseline) predicted significantly both concurrent alcohol use and increased frequency of use over the four waves (P < 0.05). This effect was entirely mediated by two forms of impulsivity, AWT and DD, both of which were characterized by underlying weakness in WM. No individual variation was observed in the slopes of WM, which suggests that individual variations in alcohol use were not associated with changes in WM in our early adolescent sample. CONCLUSIONS Early adolescent alcohol use may be a consequence of (pre-existing) weaknesses in working memory (WM) rather than a cause of it. Efforts to reduce early alcohol use should consider the distinct roles of different impulsivity dimensions, in addition to WM, as potential targets of intervention.


Journal of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics | 2001

A prospective comparison of developmental outcome of children with in utero cocaine exposure and controls using the Battelle Developmental Inventory.

Hallam Hurt; Elsa Malmud; Laura M. Betancourt; Nancy L. Brodsky; Joan M. Giannetta

Children with in utero cocaine exposure may be at risk for adverse neurodevelopmental outcome. To evaluate such outcome in young children, we administered the Battelle Developmental Inventory (BDI) to a group of inner-city children with (COC) and without (CON) in utero cocaine exposure at ages 3 and 5 years. Sixty-five COC and 68 CON, similar at age of testing, were evaluated at both time points by examiners masked to child group status. Both groups scored poorly and worsened over time. Although Total BDI raw scores were lower in the COC group than in the CON group at 3 years, this difference was related to postnatal environmental factors, caregiver (p = .022), and home environment (p = .010), not to in utero cocaine exposure (p = .88). At 5 years, the Total BDI score was related to the home environment (p < .001) but not to the caregiver (p = .36) or in utero cocaine exposure (p = .83). We conclude that inner-city children are at risk for adverse developmental outcome regardless of in utero cocaine exposure.


Frontiers in Human Neuroscience | 2012

Neighborhood disadvantage and adolescent stress reactivity

Daniel A. Hackman; Laura M. Betancourt; Nancy L. Brodsky; Hallam Hurt; Martha J. Farah

Lower socioeconomic status (SES) is associated with higher levels of life stress, which in turn affect stress physiology. SES is related to basal cortisol and diurnal change, but it is not clear if SES is associated with cortisol reactivity to stress. To address this question, we examined the relationship between two indices of SES, parental education and concentrated neighborhood disadvantage, and the cortisol reactivity of African–American adolescents to a modified version of the Trier Social Stress Test (TSST). We found that concentrated disadvantage was associated with cortisol reactivity and this relationship was moderated by gender, such that higher concentrated disadvantage predicted higher cortisol reactivity and steeper recovery in boys but not in girls. Parental education, alone or as moderated by gender, did not predict reactivity or recovery, while neither education nor concentrated disadvantage predicted estimates of baseline cortisol. This finding is consistent with animal literature showing differential vulnerability, by gender, to the effects of adverse early experience on stress regulation and the differential effects of neighborhood disadvantage in adolescent males and females. This suggests that the mechanisms underlying SES differences in brain development and particularly reactivity to environmental stressors may vary across genders.


The Journal of Pediatrics | 1997

A prospective evaluation of early language development in children with in utero cocaine exposure and in control subjects

Hallam Hurt; Elsa Malmud; Laura M. Betancourt; Nancy L. Brodsky; Joan M. Giannetta

A cohort of children of low socioeconomic status, 76 with in utero cocaine exposure and 81 control subjects, was assessed for early language development at 2 1/2 years of age by a masked examiner using the Preschool Language Scale. There were no differences between groups in expressive, receptive, or total language score (all p > or = 0.57). Physicians caring for cocaine-exposed children with early language delay should not automatically ascribe the delay to cocaine exposure but should initiate a standard evaluation for language delay.


The Journal of Pediatrics | 2008

Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging and Working Memory in Adolescents with Gestational Cocaine Exposure

Hallam Hurt; Joan M. Giannetta; Marc Korczykowski; Angela Hoang; Kathy Z. Tang; Laura M. Betancourt; Nancy L. Brodsky; David Shera; Martha J. Farah; John A. Detre

OBJECTIVE To assess the effect of gestational cocaine exposure on the prefrontal cortex (PFC) with functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI). STUDY DESIGN Using an n-back task, we obtained fMRI with a 3T Siemens scanner on 49 adolescents, 25 who were exposed to cocaine and 24 who were not exposed. The primary outcome was PFC activation during task performance. Five functionally derived regions of interest (ROI) were defined; in addition, 2 a priori anatomical ROIs were generated for Brodmann regions 10 and 46. RESULTS Of the 49 adolescents who underwent imaging, data from 17 who were exposed to cocaine and 17 who were not exposed were in the final analysis. Groups had similar performance on the n-back task (P >/= .4), with both showing a fewer number of correct responses on the 2-back than the 1-back (P < .001), indicating increased demands on working memory with greater task difficulty. In functionally derived ROIs, imaging results showed increased activation for both groups in the 2-back versus the 1-back condition. In anatomical ROIs, both groups showed greater activation in the 2-back versus the 1-back condition, with activation in the non-exposed group proportionally greater for the left prefrontal region (P = .05). CONCLUSION In this sample of adolescents, participants who were exposed to cocaine and participants who were not exposed were similar in performance on an executive function task and in fMRI activation patterns during task performance.


Neurotoxicology and Teratology | 2011

Adolescents with and without gestational cocaine exposure: Longitudinal analysis of inhibitory control, memory and receptive language☆

Laura M. Betancourt; Wei Yang; Nancy L. Brodsky; Paul R. Gallagher; Elsa Malmud; Joan M. Giannetta; Martha J. Farah; Hallam Hurt

Preclinical studies of gestational cocaine exposure (GCE) show evidence of changes in brain function at the anatomical, physiological, and behavioral levels, to include effects on developing dopaminergic systems. In contrast, human studies have produced less consistent results, with most showing small effects or no effects on developmental outcomes. Important changes in brain structure and function occur through adolescence, therefore it is possible that prenatal cocaine exposure has latent effects on neurocognitive (NC) outcome that do not manifest until adolescence or young adulthood. We examined NC function using a set of 5 tasks designed to tap 4 different systems: inhibitory control, working memory, receptive language, and incidental memory. For each NC task, data were collected longitudinally at ages 12, 14.5 and 17 years and examined using generalized estimating equations. One hundred and nine children completed at least two of the three evaluations. Covariates included in the final model were assessment number, gender, participant age at first assessment, caregiver depression, and two composites from the Home Observation for Measurement of the Environment (HOME), Environmental Stimulation and Parental Nurturance. We found no cocaine effects on inhibitory control, working memory, or receptive language (p=0.18). GCE effects were observed on incidental face memory task (p=0.055), and GCE by assessment number interaction effects were seen on the incidental word memory task (p=0.031). Participant performance on inhibitory control, working memory, and receptive language tasks improved over time. HOME Environmental Stimulation composite was associated with better receptive language functioning. With a larger sample size smaller differences between groups may have been detected. This report shows no evidence of latent effects of GCE on inhibitory control, working memory, or receptive language. GCE effects were observed on the incidental face memory task, and GCE by assessment number interaction effects was seen on the incidental word memory task.

Collaboration


Dive into the Laura M. Betancourt's collaboration.

Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Hallam Hurt

Children's Hospital of Philadelphia

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Nancy L. Brodsky

Children's Hospital of Philadelphia

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Joan M. Giannetta

Children's Hospital of Philadelphia

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Elsa Malmud

Children's Hospital of Philadelphia

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Martha J. Farah

University of Pennsylvania

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Daniel Romer

Annenberg Public Policy Center

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Daniel A. Hackman

University of Pennsylvania

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Brian B. Avants

University of Pennsylvania

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

David Shera

Children's Hospital of Philadelphia

View shared research outputs
Researchain Logo
Decentralizing Knowledge