Network


Latest external collaboration on country level. Dive into details by clicking on the dots.

Hotspot


Dive into the research topics where Margarita Svetlova is active.

Publication


Featured researches published by Margarita Svetlova.


Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience | 2012

Putting together phylogenetic and ontogenetic perspectives on empathy.

Jean Decety; Margarita Svetlova

The ontogeny of human empathy is better understood with reference to the evolutionary history of the social brain. Empathy has deep evolutionary, biochemical, and neurological underpinnings. Even the most advanced forms of empathy in humans are built on more basic forms and remain connected to core mechanisms associated with affective communication, social attachment, and parental care. In this paper, we argue that it is essential to consider empathy within a neurodevelopmental framework that recognizes both the continuities and changes in socioemotional understanding from infancy to adulthood. We bring together neuroevolutionary and developmental perspectives on the information processing and neural mechanisms underlying empathy and caring, and show that they are grounded in multiple interacting systems and processes. Moreover, empathy in humans is assisted by other abstract and domain-general high-level cognitive abilities such as executive functions, mentalizing and language, as well as the ability to differentiate anothers mental states from ones own, which expand the range of behaviors that can be driven by empathy.


Infancy | 2009

To Share or Not to Share: When Do Toddlers Respond to Another's Needs?

Celia A. Brownell; Margarita Svetlova; Sara R. Nichols

The developmental origins of sharing remain little understood. Using procedures adapted from research on prosocial behavior in chimpanzees, we presented 18- and 25-month-old children with a sharing task in which they could choose to deliver food to themselves only, or to both themselves and another person, thereby making it possible for them to share without personal sacrifice. The potential recipient, a friendly adult, was either silent about her needs and wants or made them explicit. Both younger and older toddlers chose randomly when the recipient was silent. However, when the recipient vocalized her desires 25-month-olds shared whereas younger children did not. Thus, we demonstrate that children voluntarily share valued resources with others by the end of the second year of life, but that this depends on explicit communicative cues about anothers need or desire.


Child Development | 2013

Mine or Yours? Development of Sharing in Toddlers in Relation to Ownership Understanding

Celia A. Brownell; Stephanie S. Iesue; Sara R. Nichols; Margarita Svetlova

To examine early developments in other-oriented resource sharing, fifty-one 18- and 24-month-old children were administered 6 tasks with toys or food that could be shared with an adult playmate who had none. On each task the playmate communicated her desire for the items in a series of progressively more explicit cues. Twenty-four-month-olds shared frequently and spontaneously. Eighteen-month-olds shared when given multiple opportunities and when the partner provided enough communicative support. Younger children engaged in self-focused and hypothesis-testing behavior in lieu of sharing more often than did older children. Ownership understanding, separately assessed, was positively associated with sharing and negatively associated with non-sharing behavior, independent of age and language ability.


Child Development | 2010

The Head Bone's Connected to the Neck Bone: When Do Toddlers Represent Their Own Body Topography?

Celia A. Brownell; Sara R. Nichols; Margarita Svetlova; Stephanie Zerwas; Geetha B. Ramani

Developments in very young childrens topographic representations of their own bodies were examined. Sixty-one 20- and 30-month-old children were administered tasks that indexed the ability to locate specific body parts on oneself and knowledge of how ones body parts are spatially organized, as well as body-size knowledge and self-awareness. Age differences in performance emerged for every task. Body-part localization and body spatial configuration knowledge were associated; however, body topography knowledge was not associated with body-size knowledge. Both were related to traditional measures of self-awareness, mediated by their common associations with age. It is concluded that children possess an explicit, if rudimentary, topographic representation of their own bodys shape, structure, and size by 30 months of age.


Frontiers in Psychology | 2015

Individual differences in toddlers' social understanding and prosocial behavior: Disposition or socialization?

Rebekkah L. Gross; Jesse Drummond; Emma Satlof-Bedrick; Whitney E. Waugh; Margarita Svetlova; Celia A. Brownell

We examined how individual differences in social understanding contribute to variability in early-appearing prosocial behavior. Moreover, potential sources of variability in social understanding were explored and examined as additional possible predictors of prosocial behavior. Using a multi-method approach with both observed and parent-report measures, 325 children aged 18–30 months were administered measures of social understanding (e.g., use of emotion words; self-understanding), prosocial behavior (in separate tasks measuring instrumental helping, empathic helping, and sharing, as well as parent-reported prosociality at home), temperament (fearfulness, shyness, and social fear), and parental socialization of prosocial behavior in the family. Individual differences in social understanding predicted variability in empathic helping and parent-reported prosociality, but not instrumental helping or sharing. Parental socialization of prosocial behavior was positively associated with toddlers’ social understanding, prosocial behavior at home, and instrumental helping in the lab, and negatively associated with sharing (possibly reflecting parents’ increased efforts to encourage children who were less likely to share). Further, socialization moderated the association between social understanding and prosocial behavior, such that social understanding was less predictive of prosocial behavior among children whose parents took a more active role in socializing their prosociality. None of the dimensions of temperament was associated with either social understanding or prosocial behavior. Parental socialization of prosocial behavior is thus an important source of variability in children’s early prosociality, acting in concert with early differences in social understanding, with different patterns of influence for different subtypes of prosocial behavior.


Journal of Genetic Psychology | 2010

Toddlers' understanding of peers' emotions.

Sara R. Nichols; Margarita Svetlova; Celia A. Brownell

ABSTRACT The second year of life sees dramatic developments in infants’ ability to understand emotions in adults alongside their growing interest in peers. In this study, the authors used a social-referencing paradigm to examine whether 12-, 18-, and 24-month-old children could use a peers positive or negative emotion messages about toys to regulate their own behavior with the toys. They found that 12-month-olds decreased their play with toys toward which a peer had expressed either positive or negative emotion compared with play following a peers neutral attention toward a toy. Also, 18-month-olds did not respond systematically, but 24-month-old children increased their toy play after watching a peer display negative affect toward the toy. Regardless of their age, children with siblings decreased their play with toys toward which they had seen a peer display fear, the typical social-referencing response. The authors discuss results in the context of developmental changes in social understanding and peer interaction over the second year of life.


Child Development | 2010

Toddlers' Prosocial Behavior: From Instrumental to Empathic to Altruistic Helping.

Margarita Svetlova; Sara R. Nichols; Celia A. Brownell


Infancy | 2013

Socialization of Early Prosocial Behavior: Parents' Talk About Emotions is Associated With Sharing and Helping in Toddlers

Celia A. Brownell; Margarita Svetlova; Ranita Anderson; Sara R. Nichols; Jesse Drummond


Cognitive Development | 2016

Children's developing understanding of legitimate reasons for allocating resources unequally

Marco F. H. Schmidt; Margarita Svetlova; Jana Johe; Michael Tomasello


Cognition, brain, behavior : an interdisciplinary journal | 2009

The role of social understanding and empathic disposition in young children's responsiveness to distress in parents and peers.

Sara R. Nichols; Margarita Svetlova; Celia A. Brownell

Collaboration


Dive into the Margarita Svetlova's collaboration.

Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Stephanie Zerwas

University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Jesse Drummond

University of Pittsburgh

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Researchain Logo
Decentralizing Knowledge