Marie Evans Schmidt
Center on Media and Child Health
Network
Latest external collaboration on country level. Dive into details by clicking on the dots.
Publication
Featured researches published by Marie Evans Schmidt.
The Future of Children | 2008
Marie Evans Schmidt; Elizabeth A. Vandewater
Marie Evans Schmidt and Elizabeth Vandewater review research on links between various types of electronic media and the cognitive skills of school-aged children and adolescents. One central finding of studies to date, they say, is that the content delivered by electronic media is far more influential than the media themselves.Most studies, they point out, find a small negative link between the total hours a child spends viewing TV and that childs academic achievement. But when researchers take into account characteristics of the child, such as IQ or socioeconomic status, this link typically disappears. Content appears to be crucial. Viewing educational TV is linked positively with academic achievement; viewing entertainment TV is linked negatively with achievement.When it comes to particular cognitive skills, say the authors, researchers have found that electronic media, particularly video games, can enhance visual spatial skills, such as visual tracking, mental rotation, and target localization. Gaming may also improve problem-solving skills.Researchers have yet to understand fully the issue of transfer of learning from electronic media. Studies suggest that, under some circumstances, young people are able to transfer what they learn from electronic media to other applications, but analysts are uncertain how such transfer occurs.In response to growing public concern about possible links between electronic media use and attention problems in children and adolescents, say the authors, researchers have found evidence for small positive links between heavy electronic media use and mild attention problems among young people but have found only inconsistent evidence so far for a link between attention deficit hyperactivity disorder and media use.The authors point out that although video games, interactive websites, and multimedia software programs appear to offer a variety of possible benefits for learning, there is as yet little empirical evidence to suggest that such media are more effective than other forms of instruction.
Pediatrics | 2009
Marie Evans Schmidt; Michael W. Rich; Sheryl L. Rifas-Shiman; Emily Oken; Elsie M. Taveras
OBJECTIVE. To examine the extent to which infant television viewing is associated with language and visual motor skills at 3 years of age. MEASURES. We studied 872 children who were participants in Project Viva, a prospective cohort. The design used was a longitudinal survey, and the setting was a multisite group practice in Massachusetts. At 6 months, 1 year, and 2 years, mothers reported the number of hours their children watched television in a 24-hour period, from which we derived a weighted average of daily television viewing. We used multivariable regression analyses to predict the independent associations of television viewing between birth and 2 years with Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test III and Wide-Range Assessment of Visual Motor Abilities scores at 3 years of age. RESULTS. Mean daily television viewing in infancy (birth to 2 years) was 1.2 (SD: 0.9) hours, less than has been found in other studies of this age group. Mean Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test III score at age 3 was 104.8 (SD: 14.2); mean standardized total Wide-Range Assessment of Visual Motor Abilities score at age 3 was 102.6 (SD: 11.2). After adjusting for maternal age, income, education, Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test III score, marital status, and parity, and childs age, gender, birth weight for gestational age, breastfeeding duration, race/ethnicity, primary language, and average daily sleep duration, we found that each additional hour of television viewing in infancy was not associated with Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test III or total standardized Wide-Range Assessment of Visual Motor Abilities scores at age 3. CONCLUSION. Television viewing in infancy does not seem to be associated with language or visual motor skills at 3 years of age.
Obesity | 2012
Marie Evans Schmidt; Jess Haines; Ashley O'Brien; Julia McDonald; Sarah Price; Bettylou Sherry; Elsie M. Taveras
Screen‐media use among young children is highly prevalent, disproportionately high among children from lower‐income families and racial/ethnic minorities, and may have adverse effects on obesity risk. Few systematic reviews have examined early intervention strategies to limit TV or total screen time; none have examined strategies to discourage parents from putting TVs in their childrens bedrooms or remove TVs if they are already there. In order to identify strategies to reduce TV viewing or total screen time among children <12 years of age, we conducted a systematic review of seven electronic databases to June 2011, using the terms “intervention” and “television,” “media,” or “screen time.” Peer‐reviewed intervention studies that reported frequencies of TV viewing or screen‐media use in children under age 12 were eligible for inclusion. We identified 144 studies; 47 met our inclusion criteria. Twenty‐nine achieved significant reductions in TV viewing or screen‐media use. Studies utilizing electronic TV monitoring devices, contingent feedback systems, and clinic‐based counseling were most effective. While studies have reduced screen‐media use in children, there are several research gaps, including a relative paucity of studies targeting young children (n = 13) or minorities (n = 14), limited long‐term (>6 month) follow‐up data (n = 5), and few (n = 4) targeting removing TVs from childrens bedrooms. Attention to these issues may help increase the effectiveness of existing strategies for screen time reduction and extend them to different populations.
Preventive Medicine | 2012
Elsie M. Taveras; Julia McDonald; Ashley O'Brien; Jess Haines; Bettylou Sherry; Clement J. Bottino; Karen Troncoso; Marie Evans Schmidt; Renata Koziol
OBJECTIVE To develop a home-based intervention for parents of 2-5 year old children to promote household routines to prevent overweight/obesity. METHODS We recruited 121 children from health centers in Boston between 2011 and 2012 and randomized 62 to intervention and 59 to the control condition. The 6-month intervention included 1) motivational coaching at home and by phone with a health educator, 2) mailed educational materials, and 3) weekly text messages. The intervention promoted three household routines: eating meals as a family, obtaining adequate sleep, and limiting screen time. RESULTS Of the 121 children, mean (SD) age was 4.0 (1.1) years; 52% were Hispanic, 34% Black, and 14% White/Other. Nearly 60% of the sample had annual household incomes ≤
Journal of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics | 2013
Elizabeth M. Cespedes; Julia McDonald; Jess Haines; Clement J. Bottino; Marie Evans Schmidt; Elsie M. Taveras
20,000. Approximately 64% of families reported eating together ≥ 7 times per week, however, many meals were eaten in front of a TV. Over half of the children slept less than the recommended 11h/night and 78% viewed ≥ 2 h/day of screen time. CONCLUSIONS Household routines that increase obesity risk were prevalent among low-income families in this study. If proven to be effective, promotion of household routines related to family meals, sleep, and screen time may prevent young children from becoming overweight/obese.
JAMA Pediatrics | 2013
Jess Haines; Julia McDonald; Ashley O’Brien; Bettylou Sherry; Clement J. Bottino; Marie Evans Schmidt; Elsie M. Taveras
Objective: To examine differences in obesity-related behaviors by parental US-born status among low-income, minority families participating in Healthy Habits, Happy Homes, an intervention trial to improve household routines for childhood obesity prevention. Evidence suggests lower obesity risk among adult immigrants, but research is inconclusive regarding the influence of having a non-US-born parent on childhood obesity. Method: We sampled 57 US-born and 64 non-US-born families of children aged 2 to 5.9 years living in the Boston area. At baseline, parents reported their own screen time, physical activity, diet, and sleep as well as their childrens behaviors. We used linear and logistic regression to examine the association of parental US-born status with obesity-related behaviors. Results: Mean (SD) body mass index z score was 0.94 (1.16), and it did not differ between the groups. After adjusting for parental education and child race/ethnicity, children of non-US-born (vs US-born) parents had later bedtimes (0.81 hours later; 95% confidence interval [CI], 0.37–1.25) and wake-up times (0.56 hours later; 95% CI, 0.16–0.95) and engaged in less active play (0.15 fewer hr/d; 95% CI, −0.28 to −0.01). Non-US-born (vs US-born) parents had less screen exposure. Conclusion: In this cross-section of low-income, urban families, having a parent born outside the United States was associated with a profile of risk and protective behavior; adjustment for education and race/ethnicity removed the protective associations of parental nativity with child behavior. Obesity-related differences in behaviors and home environments should be considered when designing interventions targeting low-income communities with a high proportion of non-US-born participants.
Archive | 2009
Marie Evans Schmidt; David S. Bickham; Amy Branner; Michael W. Rich
Pediatrics in Review | 2006
Marie Evans Schmidt; Michael W. Rich
Archive | 2008
Marie Evans Schmidt; Elizabeth A. Vandewater
Pediatrics in Review | 2006
Marie Evans Schmidt; Michael W. Rich