Network


Latest external collaboration on country level. Dive into details by clicking on the dots.

Hotspot


Dive into the research topics where Marina Mosquera is active.

Publication


Featured researches published by Marina Mosquera.


Nature | 2008

The first hominin of Europe

Eudald Carbonell; José María Bermúdez de Castro; J.M. Parés; Alfredo Pérez-González; Gloria Cuenca-Bescós; Andreu Ollé; Marina Mosquera; Rosa Huguet; Jan van der Made; Antonio Rosas; Robert Sala; Josep Vallverdú; Nuria García; Darryl E. Granger; María Martinón-Torres; Xosé Pedro Rodríguez; Greg M. Stock; Josep Maria Vergès; Ethel Allué; Francesc Burjachs; Isabel Cáceres; Antoni Canals; Alfonso Benito; Carlos Díez; Marina Lozano; Ana Mateos; Marta Navazo; Jesús Rodríguez; Jordi Rosell; Juan Luis Arsuaga

The earliest hominin occupation of Europe is one of the most debated topics in palaeoanthropology. However, the purportedly oldest of the Early Pleistocene sites in Eurasia lack precise age control and contain stone tools rather than human fossil remains. Here we report the discovery of a human mandible associated with an assemblage of Mode 1 lithic tools and faunal remains bearing traces of hominin processing, in stratigraphic level TE9 at the site of the Sima del Elefante, Atapuerca, Spain. Level TE9 has been dated to the Early Pleistocene (approximately 1.2–1.1 Myr), based on a combination of palaeomagnetism, cosmogenic nuclides and biostratigraphy. The Sima del Elefante site thus emerges as the oldest, most accurately dated record of human occupation in Europe, to our knowledge. The study of the human mandible suggests that the first settlement of Western Europe could be related to an early demographic expansion out of Africa. The new evidence, with previous findings in other Atapuerca sites (level TD6 from Gran Dolina), also suggests that a speciation event occurred in this extreme area of the Eurasian continent during the Early Pleistocene, initiating the hominin lineage represented by the TE9 and TD6 hominins.


Science | 1995

Lower Pleistocene hominids and artifacts from Atapuerca-TD6 (Spain)

E. Carbonell; J.M. Bermúdez de Castro; Juan Luis Arsuaga; Jc Diez; Antonio Rosas; Gloria Cuenca-Bescós; Robert Sala; Marina Mosquera; Xosé Pedro Rodríguez

Human remains dating to more than 780,000 years ago are associated with a rich faunal and lithic assemblage in the Pleistocene cave site of Gran Dolina (TD), Sierra de Atapuerca, Burgos, Spain. The micromammal species represent the late Biharian (Mimomys savini zone), and the lithic objects represent pre-Acheulean technology (Mode 1) and comes from the TD6 level below the Matuyama-Brunhes boundary. The Gran Dolina hominid fossils cannot be comfortably accommodated in any of the defined Homo species. They could be considered a primitive form of Homo heidelbergensis, but a new species might be named in the future if the sample is enlarged. The new human fossil evidence demonstrates that Western Europe was settled at least since the late early Pleistocene.


Journal of Human Evolution | 2008

A new early Pleistocene hominin mandible from Atapuerca-TD6, Spain

E. Carbonell; J.M. Bermúdez de Castro; Juan Luis Arsuaga; Ethel Allué; Markus Bastir; Amparo Benito; T. Canals; J. van der Made; Marina Mosquera; Andreu Ollé; Antonio Rosas; Jordi Rosell; Robert Sala; Josep Vallverdú

We present the description of a new mandibular specimen, ATD6-113, recovered in 2006 from the TD6 level of the Gran Dolina cave site in Sierra de Atapuerca, northern Spain. A detailed study of the lithostratigraphy of the top sequence of this level, the section from where all human remains have been recovered so far, is also presented. We have observed that the hominin stratum, previously defined as Aurora Stratum, represents a condensed deposit of at least six layers, which could not be distinguished in the test pit made in 1994-95. Therefore, the human fossil remains were probably deposited during a discrete and undetermined time period. The new mandibular fragment exhibits a very similar morphology to that of the most complete specimen, ATD6-96, which was recovered in 2003 from a different layer. This suggests that both specimens represent the same biological population. The two mandibles, as well as the small mandibular fragment ATD6-5 (which constitutes part of the holotype of Homo antecessor), present a morphological pattern clearly derived with regard to that of the African early Homo specimens usually included in H. habilis and H. rudolfensis, the mandibles D211 and D2735 from Dmanisi, and most of the early Pleistocene mandibles from Sangiran. The TD6 mandibles also exhibit some derived features with regard to the African early Pleistocene specimens included in H. ergaster (or African H. erectus). Thus, the TD6 hominins seem to represent a lineage different from other African and Asian lineages, although some (metric in particular) similarities with Chinese middle Pleistocene mandibles are noted. Interestingly, none of the apomorphic mandibular features of the European middle and early late Pleistocene hominins are present in the TD6 mandibles.


PLOS ONE | 2014

Age and Date for Early Arrival of the Acheulian in Europe (Barranc de la Boella, la Canonja, Spain)

Josep Vallverdú; Palmira Saladié; Antonio Rosas; Rosa Huguet; Isabel Cáceres; Marina Mosquera; Antonio García-Tabernero; Iván Lozano-Fernández; Antonio Pineda-Alcalá; Ángel Carrancho; Juan J. Villalaín; Didier L. Bourles; Régis Braucher; Anne Lebatard; Jaume Vilalta; Montserrat Esteban-Nadal; Maria Bennàsar; Marcus Bastir; Lucía López-Polín; Andreu Ollé; Josep Maria Vergès; Sergio Ros-Montoya; Bienvenido Martínez-Navarro; Ana Maria Garcia; Jordi Martinell; Isabel Expósito; Francesc Burjachs; Jordi Agustí; Eudald Carbonell

The first arrivals of hominin populations into Eurasia during the Early Pleistocene are currently considered to have occurred as short and poorly dated biological dispersions. Questions as to the tempo and mode of these early prehistoric settlements have given rise to debates concerning the taxonomic significance of the lithic assemblages, as trace fossils, and the geographical distribution of the technological traditions found in the Lower Palaeolithic record. Here, we report on the Barranc de la Boella site which has yielded a lithic assemblage dating to ∼1 million years ago that includes large cutting tools (LCT). We argue that distinct technological traditions coexisted in the Iberian archaeological repertoires of the late Early Pleistocene age in a similar way to the earliest sub-Saharan African artefact assemblages. These differences between stone tool assemblages may be attributed to the different chronologies of hominin dispersal events. The archaeological record of Barranc de la Boella completes the geographical distribution of LCT assemblages across southern Eurasia during the EMPT (Early-Middle Pleistocene Transition, circa 942 to 641 kyr). Up to now, chronology of the earliest European LCT assemblages is based on the abundant Palaeolithic record found in terrace river sequences which have been dated to the end of the EMPT and later. However, the findings at Barranc de la Boella suggest that early LCT lithic assemblages appeared in the SW of Europe during earlier hominin dispersal episodes before the definitive colonization of temperate Eurasia took place.


American Journal of Primatology | 2011

Population-level right-handedness for a coordinated bimanual task in naturalistic housed chimpanzees: replication and extension in 114 animals from Zambia and Spain

Miquel Llorente; David Riba; Laia Palou; Lara Carrasco; Marina Mosquera; Montserrat Colell; Olga Feliu

Recently, many studies have been conducted on manual laterality in chimpanzees. Nevertheless, whether nonhuman primates exhibit population‐level handedness remains a topic of considerable debate. One of the behaviors studied has been bimanual coordinated actions. Although recent studies have highlighted that captive chimpanzees show handedness at population level for these tasks, some authors have questioned the validity and consistency of these results. The first reason has been the humanization of the samples. The second one has been that the results refer to animals in American biomedical centers and the studies were conducted by the same team [WD Hopkins et al.]. This article aims to assess the laterality in bimanual coordination (tube task) activities in animals housed in an intermediate environment (Chimfunshi, Zambia). This has been conducted by replicating previous studies on similar samples (Mona Foundation, Spain), and then by extending the results to chimpanzees housed in intermediate settings. Individuals were evaluated through four experimental sessions (tests). Results indicated that 86% of the Chimfunshi sample was lateralized (48% RH, 38% LH). Furthermore, the sample showed population‐level right‐handedness in the mean handedness index, in Test 1, Test 2, and the first half of the study (Test 1+2). Rearing experience did not have an influence on hand preference. Taken together, the two sample (intermediate settings: Chimfunshi and Mona) results indicate a clear right‐handedness. In conclusion, this replication and extension shows that (1) the Mona and Chimfunshi chimpanzees are right‐handed in certain conditions, (2) the results are consistent with those obtained by Hopkins in captive settings, (3) the humanization of the samples does not affect manual laterality, (4) females are right‐handed at population‐level, but not males, and (5) these results reinforce the fact that the complexity of the task plays a dominant role in the expression of hand laterality among chimpanzees. Am. J. Primatol. 73:281–290, 2011.


L'Anthropologie | 2003

Les premiers comportements funéraires auraient-ils pris place à Atapuerca, il y a 350 000 ans ?

Eudald Carbonell; Marina Mosquera; Andreu Ollé; Xosé Pedro Rodríguez; Robert Sala; Josep Maria Vergès; Juan Luis Arsuaga; José María Bermúdez de Castro

Resume Pour la premiere fois, un outil lithique a ete decouvert a la Sima de los Huesos (Atapuerca, Espagne). Il s’agit d’un biface en quartzite finement taille, associe a l’ensemble d’hominides de 27 (NMI) Homo heidelbergensis, date de plus de 350 000 ans. La nature particuliere de ce depot, sa taphonomie, l’aspect paleontologique et technologique evoquent une signification symbolique a la fois de l’outil et de l’accumulation d’ossements humains.


International Journal of Primatology | 2009

Manual Laterality for Simple Reaching and Bimanual Coordinated Task in Naturalistic Housed Pan troglodytes

Miquel Llorente; Marina Mosquera; Miquel Fabré

Researchers have shown renewed interest in the study of manual lateralization in chimpanzees. Currently there is no consensus confirming the presence or absence of manual dominance at a species level, mainly for populations in the wild and in semicaptivity. We aimed to evaluate the manual laterality in a group of chimpanzees in an intermediate setting (semicaptivity) via 2 tasks: one simple and unimanual (simple reaching) and the other complex and bimanual (tube task). We replicated the same experiments from Hopkins in a new and different sample of chimpanzees. In simple reaching, the hand is used to gather food and the type of grip and the posture are evaluated. The tube task assesses the hand used to extract food from the tube and the method of extraction (digital or instrumental). Through the handedness index we observed that the subjects show clear and strong individual preferences for both tasks (100% lateralized subjects in the tube task; 86% in simple reaching), although we did not detect population preferences for any of the tasks. However, considering both tasks jointly (multiple evaluation), it was possible to detect, for the first time, skilled manual dominance at a group level in semicaptive chimpanzees in one multitask index and borderline significance in a second multitask index.


Laterality | 2007

Ethological study of manual laterality in naturalistic housed chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) from the Mona Foundation Sanctuary (Girona, Spain)

Marina Mosquera; Miquel Llorente; David Riba; Ferrán Estebaranz; Mar González-Brao; Carlos Lorenzo; Neus Sanmartí; Macarena Toll; Eudald Carbonell; Olga Feliu

During recent years, handedness of nonhuman primates has been the subject of several studies, especially focused on our closest relatives: the chimpanzees. These studies have dealt with both wild and captive chimpanzees, and they seem to point to divergent conclusions, which have been interpreted as a by-product of the human influence in the captive samples. Here we present the results of a study of 10 chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). In the past, they were trained in circus and marketing tasks (humanised behaviours), until they were confiscated and accepted into the Mona Foundation (in northeast Spain) in 2000, where they live in a semi-naturalistic environment. This study has been performed through observational bouts without systematic human influence, recording the actions carried out by chimpanzees when performing spontaneous activities. Our results indicate that chimpanzees that were under strong human influence in the past show the same trend in handedness as those living in freedom: few significant lateralities were observed among either individuals or tasks. So, laterality may not be influenced by humanisation. However, this conclusion must be taken as preliminary because very few individuals were studied.


Journal of Anthropological Research | 2013

Evidence of a neanderthal-made quartz-based technology at Navalmaíllo Rockshelter (Pinilla del Valle, Madrid region, Spain)

Belén Márquez; Marina Mosquera; Enrique Baquedano; Alfredo Pérez-González; Joaquín Panera; J.A. Espinosa; Juan Luis Arsuaga; Juan Gómez

The present work describes a preliminary study of a primarily quartz-based Mousterian lithic assemblage deposited about 75,000 years ago by Neanderthals in Navalmaíllo rockshelter (Pinilla del Valle, Madrid, Spain). Although archaeological assemblages dominated by quartz are not common in the central Iberian Peninsula, they are more common in peripheral areas such as Catalonia and Galicia. As documented in other European sites, the abundance of quartz led to its becoming the main raw material used in tool-making in the area, even though it seems to be more difficult to knap than other, more homogeneous types of rock that fracture conchoidally. Moreover, the cores found at the Navalmaíllo site appear to have been intentionally worked to a very small size, a finding also reported for other European assemblages of similar age. The other raw materials found at the site include chert, quartzite, porphyry, rock crystal, and sandstone, all of which appear to have been worked in the same manner as the quartz. The scarcity or quality of raw materials is not the reason for this behavior.


Laterality | 2014

Can hand laterality be identified through lithic technology

Amèlia Bargalló; Marina Mosquera

In this paper we present a new method for inferring handedness from lithic evidence. The study was conducted by means of an experimental programme in stone-knapping, after which the resulting lithic products (tools) were analysed. These lithic tools were produced by 15 inexpert knappers (8 right-handed and 7 left-handed), because we were not able to find a statistically significant number of left-handed expert knappers. We considered inexpert knappers to include individuals who had never struck two pebbles together, as well as individuals who were quite familiar with prehistoric tools and had had some degree of practice. The Mann-Whitney U test proved that all of them produced flakes with the same technical features, so within this sample expertise was not a factor that affected the presence or absence of the technical features analysed to determine hand laterality. The results of the experiment indicate that no single variable can be used to determine the laterality of the knapper, but rather the evidence of handedness lies in the combination of several variables. Furthermore, not all the flakes display the entire set of significant features. Therefore this study concludes that it is not possible to determine the handedness of a knapper through a single variable present on his or her flakes, but it may be possible to determine laterality by examining a combination of technical variables on a number of his or her pieces. Archaeologically, only well-preserved knapping events with numerous refitting products can be assigned to a left- or a right-handed knapper.

Collaboration


Dive into the Marina Mosquera's collaboration.

Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Andreu Ollé

Spanish National Research Council

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Eudald Carbonell

Spanish National Research Council

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Robert Sala

Spanish National Research Council

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Xosé Pedro Rodríguez

Spanish National Research Council

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Juan Luis Arsuaga

Complutense University of Madrid

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Antonio Rosas

Spanish National Research Council

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Isabel Cáceres

Spanish National Research Council

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Josep Maria Vergès

Spanish National Research Council

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Josep Vallverdú

Spanish National Research Council

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Rosa Huguet

Spanish National Research Council

View shared research outputs
Researchain Logo
Decentralizing Knowledge