Network


Latest external collaboration on country level. Dive into details by clicking on the dots.

Hotspot


Dive into the research topics where Mark D. Vida is active.

Publication


Featured researches published by Mark D. Vida.


Journal of Experimental Child Psychology | 2012

The Development of Fine-Grained Sensitivity to Eye Contact after 6 Years of Age.

Mark D. Vida; Daphne Maurer

Adults use eye contact as a cue to the mental and emotional states of others. Here, we examined developmental changes in the ability to discriminate between eye contact and averted gaze. Children (6-, 8-, 10-, and 14-year-olds) and adults (n=18/age) viewed photographs of a model fixating the center of a camera lens and a series of positions to the left/right or upward/downward and judged whether the models gaze was direct or averted to the left/right or upward/downward. The horizontal range of fixation positions leading to the perception of direct gaze (the cone of gaze) was more than 50% larger in 6-year-olds than in adults, but it was adult-like and smaller than the vertical cone of gaze by 8 years of age. The vertical cone of gaze was large and statistically adult-like by age 6, with only a small linear reduction thereafter. In all age groups, the horizontal cone of gaze was centered on the bridge of the participants nose and the vertical cone was centered slightly below the participants eye height. These findings indicate that until after age 6, relatively poor sensitivity to direct versus averted gaze limits childrens ability to use gaze cues to make social judgments.


Vision Research | 2015

Norm-based coding of facial identity in adults with autism spectrum disorder

Jennifer A. Walsh; Daphne Maurer; Mark D. Vida; Gillian Rhodes; Linda Jeffery; M. D. Rutherford

It is unclear whether reported deficits in face processing in individuals with autism spectrum disorders (ASD) can be explained by deficits in perceptual face coding mechanisms. In the current study, we examined whether adults with ASD showed evidence of norm-based opponent coding of facial identity, a perceptual process underlying the recognition of facial identity in typical adults. We began with an original face and an averaged face and then created an anti-face that differed from the averaged face in the opposite direction from the original face by a small amount (near adaptor) or a large amount (far adaptor). To test for norm-based coding, we adapted participants on different trials to the near versus far adaptor, then asked them to judge the identity of the averaged face. We varied the size of the test and adapting faces in order to reduce any contribution of low-level adaptation. Consistent with the predictions of norm-based coding, high functioning adults with ASD (n = 27) and matched typical participants (n = 28) showed identity aftereffects that were larger for the far than near adaptor. Unlike results with children with ASD, the strength of the aftereffects were similar in the two groups. This is the first study to demonstrate norm-based coding of facial identity in adults with ASD.


Neuropsychologia | 2011

Telling one face from another: Electrocortical correlates of facial characteristics among individual female faces

Xin Zheng; Catherine J. Mondloch; Mayu Nishimura; Mark D. Vida; Sidney J. Segalowitz

Research investigating the neural correlates of face processing has emphasized differences in neural activity when participants view faces versus other stimulus categories (e.g., houses). Much less is known about the neural mechanisms underlying the discrimination among individual faces. Using a large number of female faces, here we show that the amplitude of the face-sensitive N170 electrocortical component is related to a range of facial characteristics. The right N170 amplitude was related to eye color and face width. The left N170 amplitude was related to eye shape and face proportions, suggesting a functional dissociation between hemispheres. In contrast, the amplitude of the P100 and N250 components was largely unaffected by these facial characteristics. Consistent with recent findings in non-human primates, we identify for the first time evidence of human electrocortical brain potentials that are sensitive to variations in specific facial characteristics, a prerequisite for recognizing the identity of individual faces.


Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders | 2013

The influences of face inversion and facial expression on sensitivity to eye contact in high-functioning adults with autism spectrum disorders.

Mark D. Vida; Daphne Maurer; Andrew J. Calder; Gillian Rhodes; Jennifer A. Walsh; Matthew V. Pachai; M. D. Rutherford

We examined the influences of face inversion and facial expression on sensitivity to eye contact in high-functioning adults with and without an autism spectrum disorder (ASD). Participants judged the direction of gaze of angry, fearful, and neutral faces. In the typical group only, the range of directions of gaze leading to the perception of eye contact (the cone of gaze) was narrower for upright than inverted faces. In both groups, the cone of gaze was wider for angry faces than for fearful or neutral faces. These results suggest that in high-functioning adults with ASD, the perception of eye contact is not tuned to be finer for upright than inverted faces, but that information is nevertheless integrated across expression and gaze direction.


Vision Research | 2015

Adults with autism spectrum disorder show evidence of figural aftereffects with male and female faces

Jennifer A. Walsh; Mark D. Vida; Marcus Neil Morrisey; M.D. Rutherford

The norm-based coding model of face perception posits that face perception involves an implicit comparison of observed faces to a representation of an average face (prototype) that is shaped by experience. Using some methods, observers with autism spectrum disorder (ASD) have shown atypical face perception, but other methods suggest preserved face perception. Here, we used a figural aftereffects paradigm to test whether adults with ASD showed evidence of norm-based coding of faces, and whether they encode separate prototypes for male and female faces, as typical observers do. Following prolonged exposure to distorted faces that differ from their stored prototype, neurotypical adults show aftereffects: their prototype shifts in the direction of the adapting face. We measured aftereffects following adaptation to one distorted gender. There were no significant group differences in the size or direction of the aftereffects; both groups showed sex-selective aftereffects after adapting to expanded female faces but showed aftereffects for both sexes after adapting to contracted face of either sex, demonstrating that adults with and without ASD show evidence of partially dissociable male and female face prototypes. This is the first study to examine sex-selective prototypes using figural aftereffects in adults with ASD and replicates the findings of previous studies examining aftereffects in adults with ASD. The results contrast with studies reporting diminished adaptation in children with ASD.


Vision Research | 2014

Bandwidths for the perception of head orientation decrease during childhood.

Mark D. Vida; Hugh R. Wilson; Daphne Maurer

Adults use the orientation of peoples heads as a cue to the focus of their attention. We examined developmental changes in mechanisms underlying sensitivity to head orientation during childhood. Eight-, 10-, 12-year-olds, and adults were adapted to a frontal face view or a 20° left or right side view before judging the orientation of a face at or near frontal. After frontal adaptation, there were no age differences in judgments of head orientation. However, after adaptation to a 20° left or right side view, aftereffects were larger and sensitivity to head orientation was lower in 8- and 10-year-olds than in adults, with no difference between 12-year-olds and adults. A computational model indicates that these results can be modeled as a consequence of decreasing neural tuning bandwidths and decreasing additive internal noise during childhood, and/or as a consequence of increasing inhibition during childhood. These results provide the first evidence that neural mechanisms underlying sensitivity to head orientation undergo considerable refinement during childhood.


Journal of Experimental Child Psychology | 2009

Children's Representations of Facial Expression and Identity: Identity-Contingent Expression Aftereffects.

Mark D. Vida; Catherine J. Mondloch


Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders | 2014

Strategies for perceiving facial expressions in adults with autism spectrum disorder.

Jennifer A. Walsh; Mark D. Vida; M. D. Rutherford


Journal of Experimental Child Psychology | 2012

Gradual Improvement in Fine-Grained Sensitivity to Triadic Gaze after 6 Years of Age.

Mark D. Vida; Daphne Maurer


Journal of Vision | 2012

Fine-grained sensitivity to vertical differences in triadic gaze is slow to develop

Mark D. Vida; Daphne Maurer

Collaboration


Dive into the Mark D. Vida's collaboration.

Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Gillian Rhodes

University of Western Australia

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Linda Jeffery

University of Western Australia

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Researchain Logo
Decentralizing Knowledge