Network


Latest external collaboration on country level. Dive into details by clicking on the dots.

Hotspot


Dive into the research topics where Michael Potegal is active.

Publication


Featured researches published by Michael Potegal.


Physiology & Behavior | 1990

Vasopressin in the septal area of the golden hamster controls scent marking and grooming

Robert W. Irvin; Patricia Szot; Daniel M. Dorsa; Michael Potegal; Craig F. Ferris

Microinjection of arginine vasopressin into the lateral septum and bed nucleus of the stria terminalis of male hamsters stimulates intense flank marking and flank gland grooming, while microinjections of vasopressin in sites immediately adjacent to these areas or in the lateral ventricle are ineffective. Microinjections of oxytocin, angiotensin II and the behaviorally active C-terminal fragment of vasopressin, metabolite neuropeptide, by comparison, do not stimulate flank marking. Effective sites for vasopressin injection are clearly superimposable upon autoradiographically defined sites of high V1-receptor density. Furthermore, vasopressin-sensitive neurons in the lateral septum and bed nucleus of the stria terminalis are necessary for the expression of naturally elicited flank marking since the microinjection of a V1-receptor antagonist into these sites was able to temporarily block flank marking triggered by odors from conspecifics.


Psychobiology | 1983

Evidence for caudate nucleus involvement in an egocentric spatial task: Return from passive transport

Larry Abraham; Michael Potegal; S. Miller

Potegal (1972) proposed that the caudate nucleus was involved in processing sensory (especially vestibular) cues for egocentric spatial orientation. The purpose of these experiments was to determine the effect of caudate nucleus lesions on a return from passive transport (RPT) task, which has been shown to depend on vestibular input (Miller, Barnett, & Potegal, Note 1). In the first experiment, 18 male rats were trained on RPT. Six then received bilateral posterior caudate lesions, 6 received bilateral control lesions in the dorsal hippocampus, and 6 served as sham-operated controls. Postsurgical retraining revealed deficits in RPT for the caudate group, relative to hippocampal and sham groups. To test the specificity of the caudate lesion effect, a second experiment examined the effect of caudate and hippocampal lesions on an exteroceptive-cue-based spatial task, olfactory trail finding (OTF). Neither posterior caudate lesions nor dorsal hippocampal lesions produced deficits in OTF. These results suggest that the role of the posterior caudate in spatially oriented behavior is restricted to egocentric orientation based on vestibular cues. Since the caudate lesions had no effect on air-righting behavior, any vestibulostriatal contribution to RPT must be separate from the regulation of righting reflexes. This contribution may be mediated via the posterior portion of the caudate. Failure to find an effect of hippocampal lesions on RPT supports the suggestion that hippocampal involvement in spatial behavior may be restricted to memory and mapping of exteroceptive cues (O’Keefe & Nadel, 1978).


Journal of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics | 2003

Temper tantrums in young children: 1. Behavioral composition

Michael Potegal; Richard J. Davidson

&NA; Although tantrums are among the most common behavioral problems of young children and may predict future antisocial behavior, little is known about them. To develop a model of this important phenomenon of early childhood, behaviors reported in parental narratives of the tantrums of 335 children aged 18 to 60 months were encoded as present or absent in consecutive 30‐second periods. Principal Component (PC) analysis identified Anger and Distress as major, independent emotional and behavioral tantrum constituents. Anger‐related behaviors formed PCs at three levels of intensity. High‐intensity anger decreased with age, and low‐intensity anger increased. Distress, the fourth PC, consisted of whining, crying, and comfort‐seeking. Coping Style, the fifth PC, had high but opposite loadings on dropping down and running away, possibly reflecting the tendency to either “submit” or “escape.” Model validity was indicated by significant correlations of the PCs with tantrum variables that were, by design, not included in the PC analysis. J Dev Behav Pediatr 24:140‐147, 2003. Index terms: anger, crying, distress, emotion.


Psychobiology | 1983

Vestibular involvement in a passive transport and return task

Susan Miller; Michael Potegal; Larry Abraham

Rats were trained to return to a water spout after being passively transported away from it along paths containing a 90-deg turn. Path lengths were successively increased to as much as 132 cm by a modified titration procedure. The task was successfully relearned after enucleation. Subsequent vestibular nucleus damage produced a severe, lasting deficit in this task when compared with the effects of cerebellar cortex lesions. In contrast, the vestibular lesions produced only a mild, transient impairment in an olfactory localization task. The most anterior vestibular lesions also affected air righting.


Journal of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics | 2003

Temper tantrums in young children: 2. Tantrum duration and temporal organization

Michael Potegal; Michael R. Kosorok; Richard J. Davidson

&NA; This article completes the analysis of parental narratives of tantrums had by 335 children aged 18 to 60 months. Modal tantrum durations were 0.5 to 1 minute; 75% of the tantrums lasted 5 minutes or less. If the child stamped or dropped to the floor in the first 30 seconds, the tantrum was likely to be shorter and the likelihood of parental intervention less. A novel analysis of behavior probabilities that permitted grouping of tantrums of different durations converged with our previous statistically independent results to yield a model of tantrums as the expression of two independent but partially overlapping emotional and behavioral processes: Anger and Distress. Anger rises quickly, has its peak at or near the beginning of the tantrum, and declines thereafter. Crying and comfort‐seeking, components of Distress, slowly increase in probability across the tantrum. This model indicates that tantrums can provide a window on the intense emotional processes of childhood. J Dev Behav Pediatr 24:148‐154, 2003. Index terms: anger, crying, distress, emotion.


Neuropsychobiology | 1984

Wandering and parietal signs in senile dementia of Alzheimer's type.

M. J. de Leon; Michael Potegal; B. Gurland

Among a population of nursing home patients with a diagnosis of senile dementia of Alzheimers type (SDAT), a subgroup was identified who often became lost and wandered even in familiar surroundings. This subgroup differed from the rest of the SDAT patients on tests of parietal function but not on a generalized test of mental status. Thus, wandering in SDAT patients may be an indication of a specific parietal lobe involvement.


Aggressive Behavior | 1979

The reinforcing value of several types of aggressive behavior: A review

Michael Potegal

Field observations of “surplus killing” and laboratory studies of operant performance rewarded by prey-killing opportunities suggest that predatory behavior is positively reinforcing. Similarly, both repeated encounter and operant performance studies suggest that intraspecific aggression can be positively reinforcing for successful aggressors. While a few studies suggest that defensive aggression under aversive conditions may also be positively reinforcing, it appears that when appropriate response modes are available escape and/or avoidance are preferred to attack. Studies of the reinforcing properties of aggression-eliciting brain stimulation are in general agreement with these conclusions, but methodological problems with these latter observations render them less compelling. The progressive escalation of aggression seen in “warm-up effects” of birds and fish, “priming effects” of mice, and ecstatic violence of humans may be analogous processes based on the positively self-reinforcing characteristics of some kinds of aggression. The transient reductions of aggression which appear as refractory periods and satiation effects in a variety of species may reflect temporary reductions in the reinforcing value of aggression. All these temporal effects must be considered in the evaluation of experiments on the reinforcing value of aggression. More generally, it is possible that these temporal fluctuations reflect the operation of common motivational processes (aggressive states) which regulate overt aggression by changing its reinforcing value.


Neuropsychologia | 1971

A note on spatial-motor deficits in patients with Huntington's Disease: A test of a hypothesis

Michael Potegal

Abstract It has been previously demonstrated that the caudate nucleus plays a role in a system for ego-centric spatial localization. It was hypothesized that the caudate would also be involved in the compensation for self-produced movement required by such a system. A direct test of this hypothesis revealed that patients with Huntingtons Disease, which affects the caudate nucleus, were impaired in a task embodying this compensation function but were unimpaired in other equally difficult spatial-motor tasks not involving this function.


Physiology & Behavior | 1986

Bandage backfall: Labyrinthine and non-labyrinthine components

Yu Chien Chen; Sergio M. Pellis; David W. Sirkin; Michael Potegal; Philip Teitelbaum

A cataleptic animal clings in a vertical position, unmoving, for abnormally long periods by supporting some of its weight on its hindlegs, grasping with the forepaws, flexing its forelimbs, and holding the head horizontal. When the head is snugly wrapped with a bandage, the head slowly falls backward, the neck hyperextends, the forelimbs extend and the grasp is released, resulting in the animal falling backward to the ground. It was earlier suggested that in cataleptic animals, the bandage inhibits vestibular and kinesthetic mechanisms of head support, yielding the backfall sequence [35]. However, preliminary experiments showed that labyrinthectomized rats made cataleptic by haloperidol fall backwards when placed in a vertical clinging position, even without a bandage, suggesting that in the rat the bandage-backfall reaction depends only on the vestibular system. In the present paper, this result is verified but, by additional experiments, the latter conclusion is shown to be incorrect. In labyrinthectomized rats made cataleptic by other means (lateral hypothalamic damage, or bulbocapnine), backfall from clinging did not occur unless a bandage was applied. Therefore, the bandage does indeed appear to inhibit the kinesthetic mechanisms that maintain head support in labyrinthectomized cataleptic rats. Haloperidol, particularly in high doses, greatly weakens postural support in labyrinthectomized rats (causing the animal to sag down and fall back when clinging), although the effect is not detectable in rats with labyrinths intact. However, labyrinthectomy reveals that the bandage can trigger an active dorsiflexion of the neck which in itself appears to inhibit clinging and righting. Bandage-induced dorsiflexion is present to a much lesser degree in intact animals, indicating that labyrinthine mechanisms inhibit the dorsiflexion reflex. Therefore, in the intact, cataleptic rat the bandage backfall reaction appears to be produced by the combined effects of a passive component (inhibition of kinesthetic support mechanisms), and an active component (elicitation of dorsiflexion of the neck).


Behavioral Neuroscience | 1996

Brief, high-frequency stimulation of the corticomedial amygdala induces a delayed and prolonged increase of aggressiveness in male Syrian golden hamsters

Michael Potegal; M. Hebert; M. DeCoster; James L. Meyerhoff

Brief 200-Hz stimulation of the corticomedial amygdala (CMA) increases the aggressiveness of male Syrian golden hamsters for about 30 min; the effect peaks 10-15 min after stimulation. This effect is sensitive to stimulation amplitude and frequency. Stimulation at the parameters that reduce attack latency increases flank marking but does not affect copulation latency or general activity. Immunocytochemical analysis suggests that stimulation effects may be coupled to c-fos expression and that unilateral stimulation has bilateral effects. CMA stimulation effects appear to mimic part of the time course of behaviorally induced attack priming. The temporal persistence of aggression may result from long-term potentiation-like changes within CMA-related neural circuitry.

Collaboration


Dive into the Michael Potegal's collaboration.

Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Elsa Shapiro

University of Minnesota

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Kyle Rudser

University of Minnesota

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Alia Ahmed

University of Minnesota

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Brianna Yund

University of Minnesota

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

James L. Meyerhoff

Walter Reed Army Institute of Research

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Researchain Logo
Decentralizing Knowledge