Network


Latest external collaboration on country level. Dive into details by clicking on the dots.

Hotspot


Dive into the research topics where Norton W. Milgram is active.

Publication


Featured researches published by Norton W. Milgram.


Brain Research | 1983

Short-term potentiation phenomena in the rat limbic forebrain

Ronald J. Racine; Norton W. Milgram; S. Hafner

Several types of short-term postactivation potentiation (PAP) effect were examined in limbic forebrain pathways in the chronic rat. We tested 9 different stimulation sites and 1-3 target sites for each stimulation site. All pathways showed PAP effects following activation by single electrical pulses or pulse trains. Using exponential curve fitting procedures, we found that the decay curves could be best fitted by one, or a sum of 2-3, exponential curves. On the basis of time constants, these curves fell into 4 different categories: facilitation (tau = 80 ms), augmentation (tau = 7s), potentiation 1 (tau = 70s), and potentiation 2(tau = 6.5 min). The latter component was the one most reliably generated in the chronic preparation. Frequency facilitation (facilitation during a stimulation train) was also examined and it appeared to be based upon a mechanism similar to that underlying paired pulse facilitation. Evidence is presented that facilitation and augmentation may be based on the the same mechanism. The possibility that the remaining components involve different mechanisms is discussed.


Neurobiology of Aging | 1996

The canine as an animal model of human aging and dementia

Brian J. Cummings; Elizabeth Head; William W. Ruehl; Norton W. Milgram; Carl W. Cotman

The aged canine displays many features that make it an excellent model for studying the progression of pathology in brain aging and linking these findings to learning, memory and other cognitive functions. Canines develop extensive beta-amyloid deposition within neurons and their synaptic fields, which appears to give rise to senile plaques. These plaques are primarily of the early diffuse subtype. Aged canines also exhibit accumulations of lipofuscin, cerebral vascular changes, dilation of the ventricles, and cytoskeletal changes. Neurofibrillary tangles (NFTs) are not present in the aged canine. Thus, the aged canine brain provides a suitable model for studying early degeneration normally considered to be pre-Alzheimers. This supposition is also supported by behavioral data. We have found that the extent of beta-amyloid deposition correlates with a decline in select measures of cognitive function. These data provide the first evidence of a correlation between beta-amyloid accumulation and cognitive decline in the absence of NFTs. We summarize four lines of evidence that support using the aged canine as a model of human aging: (a) Aged canines develop aspects of neuropathology similar to that observed in aged humans; (b) Veterinarians have observed that many canines exhibit a clinical syndrome of age-related cognitive dysfunction; (c) Aged canines are deficient on a variety of neuropsychological tests of cognitive function; (d) The level of beta-amyloid accumulation correlates with cognitive dysfunction in the canine. These data indicate that the aged canine is a particularly useful model for studying age-related cognitive dysfunction (ARCD), early neuronal changes associated with aging, and the initial stages of senile plaque formation.


Behavioral Neuroscience | 1994

Cognitive functions and aging in the dog: acquisition of nonspatial visual tasks.

Norton W. Milgram; Elizabeth Head; Earl Weiner; Earl Thomas

Old, middle-aged, and young dogs were compared on discrimination and reversal learning and on acquisition of a delayed-nonmatching-to-sample (DNMS) test of recognition memory. DNMS acquisition was acquired more rapidly by young dogs. Reversal deficits were found between aged mixed-breed dogs and young beagles, but not between old and young beagles. Aged beagles also showed unexpected deficits in reward approach and object approach learning. Aged mixed-breed dogs did not show deficits in reward approach and object approach learning, but they learned the discrimination task more slowly than the age-matched beagles. A detailed analysis of response patterns indicated that once present, the development of side preferences contributed to deficits of old dogs in discrimination learning. In the discrimination reversal, old dogs were more persistent in responding to the previously rewarded stimulus object. Findings suggest that the dog, like other species, shows age-dependent deterioration in cognitive function, the extent of deterioration is a function of both task and previous experience, and at least part of the deterioration is a result of increased behavioral rigidity. Results also indicate that it is important to control for breed differences and previous experience.


Neurobiology of Learning and Memory | 1996

β-Amyloid Accumulation Correlates with Cognitive Dysfunction in the Aged Canine

Brian J. Cummings; Elizabeth Head; Arman J. Afagh; Norton W. Milgram; Carl W. Cotman

It is well known that beta-amyloid accumulates abnormally in Alzheimers disease; however, beta-amyloids relationship to cognitive dysfunction has not been clearly established and is often confounded by the presence of neurofibrillary tangles. We used canines to investigate the relationship between beta-amyloid accumulation and cognitive function in an animal model of aging lacking neurofibrillary tangles. The performance of 20 canines (11 purebred beagles and 9 mongrels) on a battery of six cognitive tasks was measured. These tasks included Reward Approach and Object Approach learning, as well as Discrimination, Reversal, Object Recognition, and Spatial learning and memory. Aged canines were impaired on some tasks but not others. beta-Amyloid-immunopositive plaques were found in many of the older animals. Plaques were all of the diffuse subtype and many contained intact neurons detected with double-labeling for neurofilaments. No neurofibrillary tangles were detected. beta-Amyloid was also associated with the processes of many neurons and with blood vessels. Using computerized image analysis, we quantified the area occupied by beta-amyloid in entorhinal cortex, frontal cortex, and cerebellum. Controlling for age-related increases in beta-amyloid, we observed that increased beta-amyloid deposition is strongly associated with deficits on Discrimination learning (r = .80), Reversal learning (r = .65), and Spatial learning (r = .54) but not the other tasks. There were a few differences between breeds which are discussed in the text. Overall, these data suggest that beta-amyloid deposition may be a contributing factor to age-related cognitive dysfunction prior to the onset of neurofibrillary tangle formation.


Journal of Neurochemistry | 2002

Oxidative damage increases with age in a canine model of human brain aging

Elizabeth Head; J. Liu; T. M. Hagen; Bruce A. Muggenburg; Norton W. Milgram; Bruce N. Ames; Carl W. Cotman

We assayed levels of lipid peroxidation, protein carbonyl formation, glutamine synthetase (GS) activity and both oxidized and reduced glutathione to study the link between oxidative damage, aging and β‐amyloid (Aβ) in the canine brain. The aged canine brain, a model of human brain aging, naturally develops extensive diffuse deposits of human‐type Aβ. Aβ was measured in immunostained prefrontal cortex from 19 beagle dogs (4–15 years). Increased malondialdehyde (MDA), which indicates increased lipid peroxidation, was observed in the prefrontal cortex and serum but not in cerebrospinal fluid (CSF). Oxidative damage to proteins (carbonyl formation) also increased in brain. An age‐dependent decline in GS activity, an enzyme vulnerable to oxidative damage, and in the level of glutathione (GSH) was observed in the prefrontal cortex. MDA level in serum correlated with MDA accumulation in the prefrontal cortex. Although 11/19 animals exhibited Aβ, the extent of deposition did not correlate with any of the oxidative damage measures, suggesting that each form of neuropathology accumulates in parallel with age. This evidence of widespread oxidative damage and Aβ deposition is further justification for using the canine model for studying human brain aging and neurodegenerative diseases.


Neurobiology of Aging | 2004

Learning ability in aged beagle dogs is preserved by behavioral enrichment and dietary fortification: a two-year longitudinal study

Norton W. Milgram; Elizabeth Head; Steven C. Zicker; Candace J. Ikeda-Douglas; Heather Murphey; Bruce A. Muggenburg; Christina T. Siwak; Dwight Tapp; Carl W. Cotman

The effectiveness of two interventions, dietary fortification with antioxidants and a program of behavioral enrichment, was assessed in a longitudinal study of cognitive aging in beagle dogs. A baseline protocol of cognitive testing was used to select four cognitively equivalent groups: control food-control experience (C-C), control food-enriched experience (C-E), antioxidant fortified food-control experience (A-C), and antioxidant fortified food-enriched experience(A-E). We also included two groups of young behaviorally enriched dogs, one receiving the control food and the other the fortified food. Discrimination learning and reversal was assessed after one year of treatment with a size discrimination task, and again after two years with a black/white discrimination task. The four aged groups were comparable at baseline. At one and two years, the aged combined treatment group showed more accurate learning than the other aged groups. Discrimination learning was significantly improved by behavioral enrichment. Reversal learning was improved by both behavioral enrichment and dietary fortification. By contrast, the fortified food had no effect on the young dogs. These results suggest that behavioral enrichment or dietary fortification with antioxidants over a long-duration can slow age-dependent cognitive decline, and that the two treatments together are more effective than either alone in older dogs.


Neurobiology of Aging | 1998

Visual-discrimination learning ability and β-amyloid accumulation in the dog

Elizabeth Head; Heather Callahan; Bruce A. Muggenburg; Carl W. Cotman; Norton W. Milgram

Young, middle-aged, and old beagle dogs were tested on several visual-discrimination tasks: reward- and object-approach learning, object discrimination and reversal, long-term retention of a reversal problem, and a size-discrimination task. Beta-amyloid accumulation in the entorhinal, prefrontal, parietal, and occipital cortices was quantified using immunohistochemical and imaging techniques at the conclusion of cognitive testing. Middle-aged and old dogs were impaired in size-discrimination learning. In each task, a subset of aged dogs was impaired relative to age-matched peers. Beta-amyloid accumulation was age-dependent. However, not all middle-aged and old dogs showed beta-amyloid accumulation in the entorhinal cortex. The error scores from dogs tested with a nonpreferred object during visual discrimination learning and from reversal learning were correlated with beta-amyloid in the prefrontal but not entorhinal cortex. Size-discrimination and reward and object-approach learning error scores were correlated with beta-amyloid accumulation in the entorhinal but not prefrontal cortex. The results of these studies support an association between cognitive test and the location and extent of beta-amyloid pathology.


Life Sciences | 1990

Maintenance on L-deprenyl prolongs life in aged male rats

Norton W. Milgram; Ronald J. Racine; Pamela Nellis; Antonio Mendonça; Gwen O. Ivy

The effect of l-deprenyl on longevity was examined in male Fischer rats. Subcutaneous injections of either l-deprenyl (0.25 mg/kg) or saline were given every other day starting at 23 to 25 months of age. The deprenyl-treated animals showed a significant increase in both mean and maximum survival. The differences were largest in the longest surviving animals, suggesting that an earlier onset for treatment may be beneficial. Analysis of body weights ruled out deprenyl-induced dietary restriction as an explanation for the group differences in survival. To the contrary, after about four months of treatment, the animals of l-deprenyl showed a slower rate of decrease in body weight than the controls.


Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews | 2002

Landmark discrimination learning in the dog: effects of age, an antioxidant fortified food, and cognitive strategy

Norton W. Milgram; Elizabeth Head; Bruce A. Muggenburg; D. Holowachuk; H. Murphey; Jimena Estrada; Candace J. Ikeda-Douglas; Steven C. Zicker; Carl W. Cotman

The landmark discrimination learning test can be used to assess the ability to utilize allocentric spatial information to locate targets. The present experiments examined the role of various factors on performance of a landmark discrimination learning task in beagle dogs. Experiments 1 and 2 looked at the effects of age and food composition. Experiments 3 and 4 were aimed at characterizing the cognitive strategies used in performance on this task and in long-term retention. Cognitively equivalent groups of old and young dogs were placed into either a test group maintained on food enriched with a broad-spectrum of antioxidants and mitochondrial cofactors, or a control group maintained on a complete and balanced food formulated for adult dogs. Following a wash-in period, the dogs were tested on a series of problems, in which reward was obtained when the animal responded selectively to the object closest to a thin wooden block, which served as a landmark. In Experiment 1, dogs were first trained to respond to a landmark placed directly on top of coaster, landmark 0 (L0). In the next phase of testing, the landmark was moved at successively greater distances (1, 4 or 10 cm) away from the reward object. Learning varied as a function of age group, food group, and task. The young dogs learned all of the tasks more quickly than the old dogs. The aged dogs on the enriched food learned L0 significantly more rapidly than aged dogs on control food. A higher proportion of dogs on the enriched food learned the task, when the distance was increased to 1cm. Experiment 2 showed that accuracy decreased with increased distance between the reward object and landmark, and this effect was greater in old animals. Experiment 3 showed stability of performance, despite using a novel landmark, and new locations, indicating that dogs learned the landmark concept. Experiment 4 found age impaired long-term retention of the landmark task. These results indicate that allocentric spatial learning is impaired in an age-dependent manner in dogs, and that age also affects performance when the distance between the landmark and target is increased. In addition, these results both support a role of oxidative damage in the development of age-associated cognitive dysfunction and indicate that short-term administration of a food enriched with supplemental antioxidants and mitochondrial cofactors can partially reverse the deleterious effects of aging on cognition.


Neurobiology of Aging | 2002

Dietary enrichment counteracts age-associated cognitive dysfunction in canines

Norton W. Milgram; Steven C. Zicker; Elizabeth Head; Bruce A. Muggenburg; Heather Murphey; Candace J. Ikeda-Douglas; Carl W. Cotman

Advanced age is accompanied by cognitive decline indicative of central nervous system dysfunction. One possibly critical causal factor is oxidative stress. Accordingly, we studied the effects of dietary antioxidants and age in a canine model of aging that parallels the key features of cognitive decline and neuropathology in humans. Old and young animals were placed on either a standard control food, or a food enriched with a broad spectrum of antioxidants and mitochondrial enzymatic cofactors. After 6 months of treatment, the animals were tested on four increasingly difficult oddity discrimination learning problems. The old animals learned more slowly than the young, making significantly more errors. However, this age-associated decline was reduced in the animals fed the enriched food, particularly on the more difficult tasks. These results indicate that maintenance on foods fortified with complex mixtures of antioxidants can partially counteract the deleterious effects of aging on cognition.

Collaboration


Dive into the Norton W. Milgram's collaboration.

Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Carl W. Cotman

University of California

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Bruce A. Muggenburg

Lovelace Respiratory Research Institute

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Heather Murphey

Lovelace Respiratory Research Institute

View shared research outputs
Researchain Logo
Decentralizing Knowledge