Paola Sessa
University of Padua
Network
Latest external collaboration on country level. Dive into details by clicking on the dots.
Publication
Featured researches published by Paola Sessa.
Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience | 2010
Roy Luria; Paola Sessa; Alex Gotler; Pierre Jolicœur; Roberto Dell'Acqua
Does the capacity of visual short-term memory (VSTM) depend on the complexity of the objects represented in memory? Although some previous findings indicated lower capacity for more complex stimuli, other results suggest that complexity effects arise during retrieval (due to errors in the comparison process with what is in memory) that is not related to storage limitations of VSTM, per se. We used ERPs to track neuronal activity specifically related to retention in VSTM by measuring the sustained posterior contralateral negativity during a change detection task (which required detecting if an item was changed between a memory and a test array). The sustained posterior contralateral negativity, during the retention interval, was larger for complex objects than for simple objects, suggesting that neurons mediating VSTM needed to work harder to maintain more complex objects. This, in turn, is consistent with the view that VSTM capacity depends on complexity.
European Journal of Cognitive Psychology | 2006
Pierre Jolicœur; Paola Sessa; Roberto Dell'Acqua; Nicolas Robitaille
We studied attentional control mechanisms using electrophysiological methods, focusing on the N2pc event-related potential (ERP), to track the moment-by-moment deployment of visual spatial attention. Two digits (T1 and T2, both red or both green, and masked, were embedded in a rapid serial visual presentation of letter distractors with an SOA of 200 ms or 800 ms. T1 was at fixation, whereas T2 was 3° to the left or right of fixation and presented with a concurrent equiluminant distractor digit in a different colour. T1 and T2 were reported in one block of trials, and only T2 in another block (order counterbalanced). Accuracy for T2 was lower at short SOA than at long SOA when both T1 and T2 were reported, suggesting an attentional blink (AB) effect. It was difficult to ignore T1 because T1 had the same colour as T2, producing a large deficit in T2 accuracy at short SOA in the control condition. The amplitude of the N2pc ERP component was attenuated in the short-SOA condition relative to the long-SOA condition, both in the experimental and the control conditions, suggesting that T1 involuntarily captured visual spatial attention and that while attention was deployed on T1, the processing of T2 was significantly impaired.
Frontiers in Psychology | 2010
Roberto Dell'Acqua; Paola Sessa; Francesca Peressotti; Claudio Mulatti; Eduardo Navarrete; Jonathan Grainger
We used the event-related potential (ERP) approach combined with a subtraction technique to explore the timecourse of activation of semantic and phonological representations in the picture–word interference paradigm. Subjects were exposed to to-be-named pictures superimposed on to-be-ignored semantically related, phonologically related, or unrelated words, and distinct ERP waveforms were generated time-locked to these different classes of stimuli. Difference ERP waveforms were generated in the semantic condition and in the phonological condition by subtracting ERP activity associated with unrelated picture–word stimuli from ERP activity associated with related picture–word stimuli. We measured both latency and amplitude of these difference ERP waveforms in a pre-articulatory time-window. The behavioral results showed standard interference effects in the semantic condition, and facilitatory effects in the phonological condition. The ERP results indicated a bimodal distribution of semantic effects, characterized by the extremely rapid onset (at about 100 ms) of a primary component followed by a later, distinct, component. Phonological effects in ERPs were characterized by components with later onsets and distinct scalp topography of ERP sources relative to semantic ERP components. Regression analyses revealed a covariation between semantic and phonological behavioral effect sizes and ERP component amplitudes, and no covariation between the behavioral effects and ERP component latency. The early effect of semantic distractors is thought to reflect very fast access to semantic representations from picture stimuli modulating on-going orthographic processing of distractor words.
Neuropsychologia | 2010
Roberto Dell’Acqua; Paola Sessa; Paolo Toffanin; Roy Luria; Pierre Jolicœur
We measured electroencephalographic activity during visual search of a target object among objects available to perception or among objects held in visual short-term memory (VSTM). For perceptual search, a single shape was shown first (pre-cue) followed by a search-array, and the task was to decide whether the pre-cue was or was not in the search-array. For search of VSTM, a search-array was shown first followed by a single shape (post-cue), and the task was to decide whether the post-cue was or was not in the previously displayed search-array. We focused on early lateralized electrical brain activity over posterior and temporal areas time-locked to search-arrays in pre-cue trials and to post-cues in post-cue trials. In Experiment 1, search-arrays were composed of two lateralized shapes, displayed in the upper/lower two quadrants of the monitor. In Experiment 2, search-arrays were composed of four shapes, displayed at the corners of an imaginary square centered on fixation. In pre-cue trials, we observed an N2pc of about equal amplitude and latency for search-arrays composed of two or four shapes. In post-cue trials, we observed N2pc-like activity with search-arrays composed of two shapes, that was however substantially attenuated with search-arrays composed of four shapes. For many aspects, attending to a perceptual object was functionally and neurally analogous to attending to an object held in VSTM, suggesting that spatial selective attention biases search of objects during both ongoing perception and retention.
Vision Research | 2003
Roberto Dell'Acqua; Pascali A; Pierre Jolicoeur; Paola Sessa
When two target stimuli (T1 and T2) are presented sequentially within half a second of each other, identification accuracy is often poor for T2. This phenomenon, known as attentional blink (AB), can be observed generally only if the stimulus terminating the presentation of T2 acts as an interruption mask. Recent evidence suggests that even four small dots surrounding a target item can exert masking effects, provided the target onset occurs at an unattended spatial location. In order to test whether an AB could be observed under conditions of four-dot masking of T2, five rapid serial visual presentation streams of letters were synchronously displayed on each trial of the present experiment. T1 and T2 were digits presented at unpredictable locations and unpredictable temporal intervals. T2 was followed by either a blank field, a letter, or four-dots. No AB was observed when T2 was not masked, but robust and equally sized ABs were observed when T2 was followed by both the letter mask and the four-dots.
Brain Research | 2007
Nicolas Robitaille; Pierre Jolicœur; Roberto Dell'Acqua; Paola Sessa
In order to investigate the interplay between visuo-spatial attention and central attention, we varied the relative probability (25% vs. 75%) of the responses to lateralized targets in an attentional blink paradigm. When the first target was associated with a less probable response, we observed a larger attentional blink, that is, a general reduction in accuracy for the second target. The efficiency of deployment of spatial attention to the second target was also reduced as a function of the response frequency for the first target. Both the N2pc, an event-related potential (ERP) associated with the deployment of attention in visual space, and the SPCN (sustained posterior contralateral negativity), an ERP associated with the maintenance of information in visual short-term memory, time-locked to T2 were significantly reduced when the first target was associated with a less frequent response. Furthermore, the P3 ERP to T2 was abolished when the response to T1 was rare but not when it was frequent. The results show that the association of T1 to either a rare or frequent response causes significant interference with the deployment of visual spatial attention to T2, and with the short-term consolidation of T2 into visual short-term memory.
Social Neuroscience | 2015
Federica Meconi; Jeroen Vaes; Paola Sessa
Recent studies on empathy toward other-race individuals demonstrate a preferential neural response to own-race members’ pain. Based on the observation that existing studies, using different techniques, did not provide a convergent scenario on how implicit racial prejudice relate to empathy in cross-racial contexts, in the current commentary we claim that future efforts in this domain should distinguish between processes of racial prejudice and racial stereotypes. These concepts have been differentiated in social psychology, and two independent measures have been provided to assess them. We propose that these aspects should be taken into further consideration in future studies to fully understand the social neuroscience of empathy in cross-racial contexts.
Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience | 2015
Roberto Dell'Acqua; Paul E. Dux; Brad Wyble; Mattia Doro; Paola Sessa; Federica Meconi; Pierre Jolicœur
This article explores the time course of the functional interplay between detection and encoding stages of information processing in the brain and the role they play in conscious visual perception. We employed a multitarget rapid serial visual presentation (RSVP) approach and examined the electrophysiological P3 component elicited by a target terminating an RSVP sequence. Target-locked P3 activity was detected both at frontal and parietal recording sites and an independent component analysis confirmed the presence of two distinct P3 components. The posterior P3b varied with intertarget lag, with diminished amplitude and postponed latency at short relative to long lags—an electroencephalographic signature of the attentional blink (AB). Under analogous conditions, the anterior P3a was also reduced in amplitude but did not vary in latency. Collectively, the results provide an electrophysiological record of the interaction between frontal and posterior components linked to detection (P3a) and encoding (P3b) of visual information. Our findings suggest that, although the AB delays target encoding into working memory, it does not slow down detection of a target but instead reduces the efficacy of this process. A functional characterization of P3a in attentive tasks is discussed with reference to current models of the AB phenomenon.
European Journal of Cognitive Psychology | 2006
Roberto Dell'Acqua; Pierre Jolicœur; Paola Sessa; Massimo Turatto
Brief tactile presses stimulated the index and middle fingers of the right and left hands. The stimulation on each hand consisted of a triplet of presses. Each triplet was composed of a brief press to one finger (e.g., the middle finger), followed by a brief press to the other finger (e.g., the index finger), and by a final simultaneous press to both fingers of a given hand. With equal probability, a triplet could begin with the index or middle finger, and either 360 ms or 800 ms later another triplet stimulated fingers on the other hand. The task was to indicate which finger was stimulated first in each triplet. In four experiments, response accuracy to the second triplet revealed an attentional blink in taction, that is, responses were less accurate at the short triplet–triplet interval than at the long triplet–triplet interval. This effect was substantially reduced when the first triplet could be ignored.
Frontiers in Human Neuroscience | 2014
Arianna Palmieri; Vincenzo Calvo; Johann Roland Kleinbub; Federica Meconi; Matteo Marangoni; Paolo Barilaro; Alice Broggio; Marco Sambin; Paola Sessa
The nature of near-death-experiences (NDEs) is largely unknown but recent evidence suggests the intriguing possibility that NDEs may refer to actually “perceived,” and stored, experiences (although not necessarily in relation to the external physical world). We adopted an integrated approach involving a hypnosis-based clinical protocol to improve recall and decrease memory inaccuracy together with electroencephalography (EEG) recording in order to investigate the characteristics of NDE memories and their neural markers compared to memories of both real and imagined events. We included 10 participants with NDEs, defined by the Greyson NDE scale, and 10 control subjects without NDE. Memories were assessed using the Memory Characteristics Questionnaire. Our hypnosis-based protocol increased the amount of details in the recall of all kind of memories considered (NDE, real, and imagined events). Findings showed that NDE memories were similar to real memories in terms of detail richness, self-referential, and emotional information. Moreover, NDE memories were significantly different from memories of imagined events. The pattern of EEG results indicated that real memory recall was positively associated with two memory-related frequency bands, i.e., high alpha and gamma. NDE memories were linked with theta band, a well-known marker of episodic memory. The recall of NDE memories was also related to delta band, which indexes processes such as the recollection of the past, as well as trance states, hallucinations, and other related portals to transpersonal experience. It is notable that the EEG pattern of correlations for NDE memory recall differed from the pattern for memories of imagined events. In conclusion, our findings suggest that, at a phenomenological level, NDE memories cannot be considered equivalent to imagined memories, and at a neural level, NDE memories are stored as episodic memories of events experienced in a peculiar state of consciousness.