Network


Latest external collaboration on country level. Dive into details by clicking on the dots.

Hotspot


Dive into the research topics where Pasquale Rinaldi is active.

Publication


Featured researches published by Pasquale Rinaldi.


Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education | 2009

Lexical and Grammatical Abilities in Deaf Italian Preschoolers: The Role of Duration of Formal Language Experience

Pasquale Rinaldi; Cristina Caselli

We evaluated language development in deaf Italian preschoolers with hearing parents, taking into account the duration of formal language experience (i.e., the time elapsed since wearing a hearing aid and beginning language education) and different methods of language education. Twenty deaf children were matched with 20 hearing children for age and with another 20 hearing children for duration of experience. Deaf children showed a significant delay in both vocabulary and grammar when compared to same-age hearing children yet a similar development compared to hearing children matched for duration of formal language experience. The delay in linguistic development could be attributable to shorter formal language experience and not to deafness itself. Deaf children exposed to spoken language accompanied by signs tended to understand and produce more words than children exposed only to spoken language. We suggest that deaf children be evaluated based on their linguistic experience and cognitive and communicative potential.


International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders | 2013

Linguistic and Pragmatic Skills in Toddlers with Cochlear Implant.

Pasquale Rinaldi; Francesca Baruffaldi; Sandro Burdo; Maria Cristina Caselli

BACKGROUND An increasing number of deaf children received cochlear implants (CI) in the first years of life, but no study has focused on linguistic and pragmatic skills in children with CI younger than 3 years of age. AIMS To estimate the percentage of children who had received a CI before 2 years of age whose linguistic skills were within the normal range; to compare linguistic skills of children implanted by 12 months of age with children implanted between 13 and 26 months of age; and to describe the relationship among lexical, grammar and pragmatic skills. METHODS & PROCEDURES The participants consisted of children who were included on the patient lists of the Service of Audio-Vestibology of the Circolo Hospital in Varese, Italy, and met the following criteria: chronological age between 18 and 36 months; CI activated between 8 and 30 months of age; absence of other reported deficits; hearing parents; and not less than 6 months of CI experience. Language development was evaluated through MacArthur-Bates CDI; pragmatic skills (assertiveness and responsiveness) were evaluated through the Social Conversational Skills Rating Scale. The scores obtained were transformed into z-scores and compared with normative data. The relationship among lexical, grammar and pragmatic skills were tested using Spearman Rho correlations. Children with CI were divided into groups based on the age at CI activation and the differences between the two groups were tested using the Students t-test. OUTCOMES & RESULTS Data from 23 deaf children were collected. Fewer than half of the children were within the normal range for lexical production and use of sentences; more than one-third of them fell below the normal range for both lexical and grammar skills. No significant difference was found in vocabulary size or early grammar skills when comparing children who received the CI by 12 months of age with those implanted during the second year of life. Despite the strong relationship among lexical, grammar and pragmatic skills, the delays found for grammar and pragmatic skills were greater than expected based on the vocabulary size. Age at diagnosis of hearing loss was the only predictor of vocabulary size. CONCLUSIONS & IMPLICATIONS CI may provide deaf children with a good opportunity to develop language skills, but severe difficulties in early social experiences and interaction mediated by language still remain. Delays in these aspects suggest that interventions improving pragmatic skills are recommended even on very young children with CI.


Behavior Research Methods Instruments & Computers | 2004

A database for semantic, grammatical, and frequency properties of the first words acquired by Italian children

Pasquale Rinaldi; Laura Barca; Cristina Burani

The CFVlexvar.xls database includes imageability, frequency, and grammatical properties of the first words acquired by Italian children. For each of 519 words that are known by children 18–30 months of age (taken from Caselli & Casadio’s, 1995, Italian version of the MacArthur Communicative Development Inventory), new values of imageability are provided and values for age of acquisition, child written frequency, and adult written and spoken frequency are included. In this article, correlations among the variables are discussed and the words are grouped into grammatical categories. The results show that words acquired early have imageable referents, are frequently used in the texts read and written by elementary school children, and are frequent in adult written and spoken language. Nouns are acquired earlier and are more imageable than both verbs and adjectives. The composition in grammatical categories of the child’s first vocabulary reflects the composition of adult vocabulary. The full set of these norms can be downloaded fromwww.psychonomic.org/archive/.


Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education | 2014

Sign Vocabulary in Deaf Toddlers Exposed to Sign Language Since Birth

Pasquale Rinaldi; Maria Cristina Caselli; Alessio Di Renzo; Tiziana Gulli; Virginia Volterra

Lexical comprehension and production is directly evaluated for the first time in deaf signing children below the age of 3 years. A Picture Naming Task was administered to 8 deaf signing toddlers (aged 2-3 years) who were exposed to Sign Language since birth. Results were compared with data of hearing speaking controls. In both deaf and hearing children, comprehension was significantly higher than production. The deaf group provided a significantly lower number of correct responses in production than did the hearing controls, whereas in comprehension, the 2 groups did not differ. Difficulty and ease of items in comprehension and production was similar for signing deaf children and hearing speaking children, showing that, despite size differences, semantic development followed similar paths. For signing children, predicates production appears easier than nominals production compared with hearing children acquiring spoken language. Findings take into account differences in input modalities and language structures.


Bilingualism: Language and Cognition | 2014

Language development in a bimodal bilingual child with cochlear implant: A longitudinal study

Pasquale Rinaldi; Maria Cristina Caselli

To address the negative effects of deafness on spoken language acquisition, many clinicians suggest using cochlear implant (CI) and oral education and advise against sign language, even when combined with spoken language (i.e., bilingualism), believing that it may slow down spoken language development. In a deaf child with CI who was exposed at an early age to Italian Sign Language and spoken Italian, we evaluated language development and the relationship between the two languages. The number of words/signs produced by the child consistently increased with age, and the vocabulary growth rate in spoken Italian was equivalent to that of hearing peers. Before CI, the child relied almost exclusively on sign language; after CI, he gradually shifted to spoken Italian yet still used sign language when unable to retrieve words in spoken Italian. We conclude that bimodal bilingualism may scaffold the development of spoken language also in deaf children with CI.


Brain and Cognition | 2015

Finding the balance between capture and control: Oculomotor selection in early deaf adults

Benedetta Heimler; Wieske van Zoest; Francesca Baruffaldi; Mieke Donk; Pasquale Rinaldi; Maria Cristina Caselli; Francesco Pavani

Previous work investigating the consequence of bilateral deafness on attentional selection suggests that experience-dependent changes in this population may result in increased automatic processing of stimulus-driven visual information (e.g., saliency). However, adaptive behavior also requires observers to prioritize goal-driven information relevant to the task at hand. In order to investigate whether auditory deprivation alters the balance between these two components of attentional selection, we assessed the time-course of overt visual selection in deaf adults. Twenty early-deaf adults and twenty hearing controls performed an oculomotor additional singleton paradigm. Participants made a speeded eye-movement to a unique orientation target, embedded among homogenous non-targets and one additional unique orientation distractor that was more, equally or less salient than the target. Saliency was manipulated through color. For deaf participants proficiency in sign language was assessed. Overall, results showed that fast initiated saccades were saliency-driven, whereas later initiated saccades were goal-driven. However, deaf participants were overall slower than hearing controls at initiating saccades and also less captured by task-irrelevant salient distractors. The delayed oculomotor behavior of deaf adults was not explained by any of the linguistic measures acquired. Importantly, a multinomial model applied to the data revealed a comparable evolution over time of the underlying saliency- and goal-driven processes between the two groups, confirming the crucial role of saccadic latencies in determining the outcome of visual selection performance. The present findings indicate that prioritization of saliency-driven information is not an unavoidable phenomenon in deafness. Possible neural correlates of the documented behavioral effect are also discussed.


Acta Psychologica | 2013

Bilingual vocabulary size and lexical reading in Italian.

Silvia Primativo; Pasquale Rinaldi; Shaunna O'Brien; Despina Paizi; Lisa S. Arduino; Cristina Burani

In the present study we investigated how the vocabulary size of English-Italian bilinguals affects reading aloud in Italian (L2) modulating the readers sensitivity to lexical aspects of the language. We divided adult bilinguals in two groups according to their vocabulary size (Larger - LV, and smaller - SV), and compared their naming performance to that of native Italian (NI) readers. In Experiment 1 we investigated the lexicality and word frequency effects in reading aloud. Similarly to NI, both groups of bilinguals showed these effects. In Experiment 2 we investigated stress assignment - which is not predictable by rule - to Italian words. The SV group made more stress errors in reading words with a non-dominant stress pattern compared to the LV group. The results suggest that the size of the readers L2 lexicon affects the probability of correct reading aloud. Overall, the results indicate that proficient adult bilinguals show a similar sensibility to the statistical and distributional properties of the language as compared to Italian monolinguals.


PLOS ONE | 2013

Visual word recognition in deaf readers: lexicality is modulated by communication mode.

Laura Barca; Giovanni Pezzulo; Marianna Castrataro; Pasquale Rinaldi; Maria Cristina Caselli

Evidence indicates that adequate phonological abilities are necessary to develop proficient reading skills and that later in life phonology also has a role in the covert visual word recognition of expert readers. Impairments of acoustic perception, such as deafness, can lead to atypical phonological representations of written words and letters, which in turn can affect reading proficiency. Here, we report an experiment in which young adults with different levels of acoustic perception (i.e., hearing and deaf individuals) and different modes of communication (i.e., hearing individuals using spoken language, deaf individuals with a preference for sign language, and deaf individuals using the oral modality with less or no competence in sign language) performed a visual lexical decision task, which consisted of categorizing real words and consonant strings. The lexicality effect was restricted to deaf signers who responded faster to real words than consonant strings, showing over-reliance on whole word lexical processing of stimuli. No effect of stimulus type was found in deaf individuals using the oral modality or in hearing individuals. Thus, mode of communication modulates the lexicality effect. This suggests that learning a sign language during development shapes visuo-motor representations of words, which are tuned to the actions used to express them (phono-articulatory movements vs. hand movements) and to associated perceptions. As these visuo-motor representations are elicited during on-line linguistic processing and can overlap with the perceptual-motor processes required to execute the task, they can potentially produce interference or facilitation effects.


Multisensory Research | 2013

The impact of saliency on overt visual selection in early-deaf adults

Benedetta Heimler; Francesca Baruffaldi; Wieske van Zoest; Pasquale Rinaldi; Maria Cristina Caselli; Francesco Pavani

Following bilateral deafness, vision reorganizes to overcome the lack of audition. Results from covert attention studies show enhanced reactivity and rapid attention orienting to abrupt visual stimuli in deaf people, suggesting that crossmodal changes may prioritize saliency-driven attention capture in this population. However, deaf individuals also need to maintain goal-directed behaviors. We investigated the time-course of overt visual selection in deaf adults, to assess when in time deaf individuals implement efficient top-down control over saliency-driven search. Eighteen early-deaf adults and sixteen hearing controls performed an oculomotor additional singleton paradigm. For deaf participants language abilities in oral and sign languages were assessed. Participants made a speeded saccadic eye movement to a unique orientation singleton. The target was presented among homogenous non-targets and one additional orientation singleton that was more, equally or less salient than the target. Results showed a similar pattern of performance in the two groups: fast initiated saccades were saliency-driven whereas later initiated saccades were more goal-driven. However, deaf were overall slower than hearing participants at initiating saccades. This delay in saccadic latencies was driven by a sub-group of deaf adults, who were able to bypass saliency capture by withholding their saccades until target selection was efficiently achieved. No correlation with linguistic abilities emerged, showing no role of sign language acquisition in this behavior. Taken together, the present findings reveal that overt visual reorganization in deaf adults does not drastically alter the interplay between bottom-up and top-down strategies. If anything, it prioritizes the implementation of efficient goal-directed behaviors.


Journal of Speech Language and Hearing Research | 2012

Cochlear implant in the second year of life: lexical and grammatical outcomes.

Maria Cristina Caselli; Pasquale Rinaldi; Cristiana Varuzza; Anna Giuliani; Sandro Burdo

Collaboration


Dive into the Pasquale Rinaldi's collaboration.

Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Laura Barca

National Research Council

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Laura Sparaci

National Research Council

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Olga Capirci

National Research Council

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Tommaso Lucioli

National Research Council

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Researchain Logo
Decentralizing Knowledge