Rachel Pulverman
Delaware State University
Network
Latest external collaboration on country level. Dive into details by clicking on the dots.
Publication
Featured researches published by Rachel Pulverman.
Cognition | 2010
Mandy J. Maguire; Kathy Hirsh-Pasek; Roberta Michnick Golinkoff; Mutsumi Imai; Etsuko Haryu; Sandra B. Vanegas; Hiroyuki Okada; Rachel Pulverman; Brenda Sanchez-Davis
The worlds languages draw on a common set of event components for their verb systems. Yet, these components are differentially distributed across languages. At what age do children begin to use language-specific patterns to narrow possible verb meanings? English-, Japanese-, and Spanish-speaking adults, toddlers, and preschoolers were shown videos of an animated star performing a novel manner along a novel path paired with a language-appropriate nonsense verb. They were then asked to extend that verb to either the same manner or the same path as in training. Across languages, toddlers (2- and 2.5-year-olds) revealed a significant preference for interpreting the verb as a path verb. In preschool (3- and 5-year-olds) and adulthood, the participants displayed language-specific patterns of verb construal. These findings illuminate the way in which verb construal comes to reflect the properties of the input language.
Cognition | 2008
Rachel Pulverman; Roberta Michnick Golinkoff; Kathy Hirsh-Pasek; Jennifer Sootsman Buresh
Do 14- to 17-month-olds notice the paths and manners of motion events? English- and Spanish-learning infants were habituated to an animated motion event including a manner (e.g., spinning) and a path (e.g., over). They were then tested on four types of events that changed either the manner, the path, both, or neither component. Both English- and Spanish-learning infants attended to changes of manner and changes of path. Thus, infants from two different language communities proved sensitive to components of events that undergird relational term learning.
Child Development | 2013
Rachel Pulverman; Lulu Song; Kathy Hirsh-Pasek; Shannon M. Pruden; Roberta Michnick Golinkoff
In the world, the manners and paths of motion events take place together, but in language, these features are expressed separately. How do infants learn to process motion events in linguistically appropriate ways? Forty-six English-learning 7- to 9-month-olds were habituated to a motion event in which a character performed both a manner and a path, and then tested on events that changed the manner, path, both, or neither. Infants detected each type of change, but only the girls showed evidence of processing manner and path as independent features. This gender difference provides clues about the universal development of manner and path concepts from more basic perceptual skills. Results have implications for how representations of linguistically relevant semantic elements develop conceptually.
Language Learning and Development | 2016
Lulu Song; Rachel Pulverman; Christina Pepe; Roberta Michnick Golinkoff; Kathy Hirsh-Pasek
ABSTRACT Learning a language is more than learning its vocabulary and grammar. For example, compared with English, Spanish uses many more path verbs such as ascender (‘to move upward’) and salir (‘to go out’), and expresses manner of motion optionally. English, in contrast, has many manner verbs (e.g., run, jog) and expresses path in prepositional phrases (e.g., out of the barn). The way in which a language encodes an event is known as its lexicalization pattern or bias. Using a written sentence elicitation task, we asked whether adult Spanish learners whose L1 was English adopted Spanish lexicalization biases, and what types of L2 exposure facilitated the learning of lexicalization biases. Results showed that advanced, but not intermediate, adult Spanish learners showed a path bias comparable to that found in native speakers of Spanish. Furthermore, study abroad experience is associated with better acquisition of L2 lexicalization biases when describing certain types of events.
Behavioral and Brain Sciences | 2001
Kathy Hirsh-Pasek; Elizabeth A. Hennon; Roberta Michnick Golinkoff; Khara Pence; Rachel Pulverman; Jenny Sootsman; Shannon M. Pruden; Mandy J. Maguire
Blooms eloquent and comprehensive treatment of early word learning holds that social intention is foundational for language development. While we generally support his thesis, we call into question two of his proposals: (1) that attention to social information in the environment implies social intent, and (2) that infants are sensitive to social intent at the very beginnings of word learning.
Archive | 2006
Rachel Pulverman; Kathy Hirsh-Pasek; Roberta Michnick Golinkoff; Shannon M. Pruden; Sara J. Salkind
Developmental Psychology | 2011
Cheri C. Y. Chan; Twila Tardif; Jie Chen; Rachel Pulverman; Liqi Zhu; Xiangzhi Meng
Developmental Psychology | 2015
Jie Chen; Twila Tardif; Rachel Pulverman; Marianella Casasola; Liqi Zhu; Xiaobei Zheng; Xiangzhi Meng
Archive | 2007
Amanda Brandone; Roberta Michnick Golinkoff; Rachel Pulverman; Mandy J. Maguire; Kathy Hirsh-Pasek; Shannon M. Pruden
international conference on development and learning | 2009
Jie Chen; Cheri Chan; Rachel Pulverman; Twila Tardif; Marianella Casasola; Xiaobei Zheng; Xiangzhi Meng