Network


Latest external collaboration on country level. Dive into details by clicking on the dots.

Hotspot


Dive into the research topics where Robert L. Bornschein is active.

Publication


Featured researches published by Robert L. Bornschein.


Environmental Health Perspectives | 2005

Low-Level Environmental Lead Exposure and Children's Intellectual Function: An International Pooled Analysis

Bruce P. Lanphear; Richard Hornung; Jane Khoury; Kimberly Yolton; Peter Baghurst; David C. Bellinger; Richard L. Canfield; Kim N. Dietrich; Robert L. Bornschein; Tom Greene; Stephen J. Rothenberg; Herbert L. Needleman; Lourdes Schnaas; Gail A. Wasserman; Joseph H. Graziano; Russell Roberts

Lead is a confirmed neurotoxin, but questions remain about lead-associated intellectual deficits at blood lead levels < 10 μg/dL and whether lower exposures are, for a given change in exposure, associated with greater deficits. The objective of this study was to examine the association of intelligence test scores and blood lead concentration, especially for children who had maximal measured blood lead levels < 10 μg/dL. We examined data collected from 1,333 children who participated in seven international population-based longitudinal cohort studies, followed from birth or infancy until 5–10 years of age. The full-scale IQ score was the primary outcome measure. The geometric mean blood lead concentration of the children peaked at 17.8 μg/dL and declined to 9.4 μg/dL by 5–7 years of age; 244 (18%) children had a maximal blood lead concentration < 10 μg/dL, and 103 (8%) had a maximal blood lead concentration < 7.5 μg/dL. After adjustment for covariates, we found an inverse relationship between blood lead concentration and IQ score. Using a log-linear model, we found a 6.9 IQ point decrement [95% confidence interval (CI), 4.2–9.4] associated with an increase in concurrent blood lead levels from 2.4 to 30 μg/dL. The estimated IQ point decrements associated with an increase in blood lead from 2.4 to 10 μg/dL, 10 to 20 μg/dL, and 20 to 30 μg/dL were 3.9 (95% CI, 2.4–5.3), 1.9 (95% CI, 1.2–2.6), and 1.1 (95% CI, 0.7–1.5), respectively. For a given increase in blood lead, the lead-associated intellectual decrement for children with a maximal blood lead level < 7.5 μg/dL was significantly greater than that observed for those with a maximal blood lead level ≥7.5 μg/dL (p = 0.015). We conclude that environmental lead exposure in children who have maximal blood lead levels < 7.5 μg/dL is associated with intellectual deficits.


Neurotoxicology and Teratology | 2001

Early exposure to lead and juvenile delinquency.

Kim N. Dietrich; Ris M. Douglas; Paul Succop; Omer G. Berger; Robert L. Bornschein

Cross-sectional studies have reported an association between lead (Pb) levels in bone and delinquent behavior in later childhood and adolescence. This is the first prospective longitudinal study of Pb and child development to address this question with comprehensive assessments of toxicant exposure and other developmental cofactors. A prospective longitudinal birth cohort of 195 urban, inner-city adolescents recruited between 1979 and 1985 was examined. Relationships between prenatal and postnatal exposure to Pb (serial blood Pb determinations) and antisocial and delinquent behaviors (self- and parental reports) were examined. Prenatal exposure to Pb was significantly associated with a covariate-adjusted increase in the frequency of parent-reported delinquent and antisocial behaviors, while prenatal and postnatal exposure to Pb was significantly associated with a covariate-adjusted increase in frequency of self-reported delinquent and antisocial behaviors, including marijuana use. Use of marijuana itself by Cincinnati Lead Study (CLS) teens was strongly associated with all measures of delinquent and antisocial behavior. This prospective longitudinal study confirmed earlier clinical observations and recent retrospective studies that have linked Pb exposure with antisocial behavior in children and adolescents. Both prenatal and postnatal exposure to Pb were associated with reported antisocial acts and may play a measurable role in the epigenesis of behavioral problems independent of the other social and biomedical cofactors assessed in this study.


Neurotoxicology and Teratology | 1993

The developmental consequences of low to moderate prenatal and postnatal lead exposure: intellectual attainment in the Cincinnati Lead Study Cohort following school entry.

Kim N. Dietrich; Omer G. Berger; Paul Succop; Paul B. Hammond; Robert L. Bornschein

In a further follow-up study of the Cincinnati Lead Study Cohort, 253 children were administered the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children-Revised (WISC-R) at approximately 6.5 years of age. Postnatal blood lead concentrations were inversely associated with Full-Scale (FSIQ) and Performance IQ (PIQ). Following statistical adjustment for developmental co-factors such as maternal IQ and an assessment of the quality of caretaking in the home environment, a statistically significant relationship remained between postnatal blood lead concentrations and PIQ. Further statistical analyses suggested that averaged lifetime blood lead concentrations in excess of 20 micrograms/dL were associated with deficits in PIQ on the order of approximately 7 points when compared to children with mean concentrations less or equal to 10 micrograms/dL. These results are discussed in terms of their consistency with other similar studies as well as their internal consistency with earlier reports on this cohort. The findings of this investigation support recent initiatives in the United States to reduce the exposure of children to environmental lead.


Neurotoxicology and Teratology | 1991

Lead exposure and the cognitive development of urban preschool children: The cincinnati lead study cohort at age 4 years

Kim N. Dietrich; Paul Succop; Omer G. Berger; Paul B. Hammond; Robert L. Bornschein

The purpose of this analysis was to determine if significant associations could be observed between prenatal/postnatal blood lead (PbB) levels and the cognitive development of 258 urban, inner-city children at 4 years of age. These children have been followed since birth with frequent assessments of general health, PbB, and neuropsychological status. The Kaufman Assessment Battery for Children (K-ABC) was administered at approximately 4 years of age. Higher neonatal PbB levels were associated with poorer performance on all K-ABC subscales. However, this inverse association was limited to children from the poorest families. Maternal PbB levels were unrelated to 4-year cognitive status. Few statistically significant associations between postnatal PbB levels and K-ABC scales could be found. However, the results did suggest a weak inverse relationship between postnatal PbB levels and performance on a K-ABC subscale which assesses visual-spatial and visual-motor integration skills. In these results we note both contradiction and accord with previously published prospective studies.


Environmental Research | 1985

Evolution of efficient methods to sample lead sources, such as house dust and hand dust, in the homes of children

Shane S. Que Hee; Belinda Peace; C. Scott Clark; James R. Boyle; Robert L. Bornschein; P.B. Hammond

Efficient sampling methods to recover lead-containing house dust and hand dust have been evolved so that sufficient lead is collected for analysis, and to ensure that correlational analyses linking these two parameters to blood lead are not dependent on the efficiency of sampling. Precise collection of loose house dust from a 1-unit area (484 cm2) with a Tygon or stainless steel sampling tube connected to a portable sampling pump (1.2 to 2.5 liters/min) required repetitive sampling (three times). The Tygon tube sampling technique for loose house dust less than 177 microns in diameter was around 72% efficient with respect to dust weight and lead collection. A representative house dust contained 81% of its total weight in this fraction. A single handwipe for applied loose hand dust was not acceptably efficient or precise, and at least three wipes were necessary to achieve recoveries of greater than 80% of the lead applied. House dusts of different particle sizes less than 246 microns adhered equally well to hands. Analysis of lead-containing material usually required at least three digestions/decantations using hot plate or microwave techniques to allow at least 90% of the lead to be recovered. It was recommended that other investigators validate their handwiping, house dust sampling, and digestion techniques to facilitate comparison of results across studies. The final methodology for the Cincinnati longitudinal study was three sampling passes for surface dust using a stainless steel sampling tube; three microwave digestions/decantations for analysis of dust and paint; and three wipes with handwipes with one digestion/decantation for the analysis of six handwipes together.


Journal of The International Neuropsychological Society | 2004

Early exposure to lead and neuropsychological outcome in adolescence

M. Douglas Ris; Kim N. Dietrich; Paul Succop; Omer G. Berger; Robert L. Bornschein

One hundred and ninety-five participants in the Cincinnati Lead Study were neuropsychologically evaluated in mid-adolescence. The neuropsychological measures yielded five factors labeled Memory, Learning/IQ, Attention, Visuoconstruction, and Fine-Motor. Prenatal, Average Childhood, and 78 month blood lead (PbB) levels were used in a series of multiple regression analyses. Following rigorous covariate pretesting and adjustment, a significant main effect of 78 month PbB on the Fine-Motor factor was found (p <.004). Significant interactions were also found between gender and lead exposure parameters for both Attention and Visuoconstruction indicating heightened risk in males. Finally, a trend toward significance was found for the PbB x SES interaction for Learning/IQ, consistent with previous evidence of increased educational and cognitive vulnerability for youth from more disadvantaged backgrounds. These results provide new evidence from the longest continuing prospective study of the remote effects of early lead exposure. They indicate the presence of selective neuropsychological effects in this population, and also that males and females are not uniformly affected. These results also underscore the complexity of models of neurobehavioral development, and the modest predictive power of any single determinant.


Environmental Research | 1985

Condition and type of housing as an indicator of potential environmental lead exposure and pediatric blood lead levels

C.S. Clark; Robert L. Bornschein; Paul Succop; S.S.Que Hee; P.B. Hammond; Belinda Peace

Environmental evaluations in a prospective behavioral study of children with blood lead levels up to about 50 micrograms/dl were performed by an intensive environmental survey and by exterior visual evaluation of housing quality. Serial blood lead values for infants in the study were compared to exterior housing type and quality, which itself was also compared with results of the intensive environmental evaluation. Five housing condition and type categories were defined: public housing; private housing (satisfactory, deteriorated, and dilapidated); and rehabilitated private housing. In this interim report on the first subset of available data, the housing categories were found to differ in paint and environment dust lead levels, with public and rehabilitated housing having lowest values. Blood lead concentrations of children differed across housing categories as early as 6 months of age, with children residing in public housing having lowest levels, followed by those in rehabilitated housing. The spread in mean blood lead concentrations among the housing quality categories increased with increasing age of the children. Housing category accounted for over one-half of the blood lead variability in 18-month-old children.


Environmental Research | 1985

THE INFLUENCE OF SOCIAL AND ENVIRONMENTAL FACTORS ON DUST LEAD, HAND LEAD AND BLOOD LEAD LEVELS IN YOUNG CHILDREN

Robert L. Bornschein; Paul Succop; Kim N. Dietrich; C.S. Clark; S.Que Hee; P.B. Hammond

The roles of environmental and behavioral factors in determining blood levels were studied in a cohort of young children living in an urban environment. The subjects were observed at 3-month intervals from birth to 24 months of age. Repeated measurements were made of the childrens blood lead levels, environmental levels of lead in house dust, and in the dust found on the childrens hands. A qualitative rating of the residence and of the socioeconomic status of the family was obtained. Interviews and direct observation of parent and child at home were used to evaluate various aspects of caretaker-child interactions. Data analysis consisted of a comparison of results obtained by simple correlational analysis, multiple regression analysis, and structural equations analysis. The results demonstrated that structural equation modeling offers a useful approach to unraveling the complex interactions present in the data set. In this preliminary analysis, the suspected relationship between the levels of lead in house dust and on hands and the blood lead level was clearly demonstrated. Furthermore, the analyses indicated an important interplay between environmental sources and social factors in the determination of hand lead and blood lead levels in very young children.


Critical Reviews in Toxicology | 1980

Behavioral effects of moderate lead exposure in children and animal models: Part 1, clinical studies

Robert L. Bornschein; Douglas Pearson; Lawrence W. Reiter; Lester D. Grant

(1980). Behavioral effects of moderate lead exposure in children and animal models: Part 1, clinical studies. CRC Critical Reviews in Toxicology: Vol. 8, No. 1, pp. 43-99.


Chemical Speciation and Bioavailability | 1991

Urban Lead Exposures of Children in Cincinnati, Ohio

Scott Clark; Robert L. Bornschein; Paul Succop; Sandy Roda; Belinda Peace

AbstractEnvironmental dust lead and other lead measures were highly intercorrelated for the wide range of housing in the Cincinnati prospective study. The causal pathway revealed by the data (soil ...

Collaboration


Dive into the Robert L. Bornschein's collaboration.

Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Paul Succop

University of Cincinnati

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Kim N. Dietrich

University of Cincinnati Academic Health Center

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Scott Clark

University of Cincinnati

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Omer G. Berger

University of Cincinnati Academic Health Center

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Rakesh Shukla

University of Cincinnati

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Paul B. Hammond

University of Cincinnati Academic Health Center

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Sandy Roda

University of Cincinnati

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

P.B. Hammond

University of Cincinnati

View shared research outputs
Researchain Logo
Decentralizing Knowledge