Network


Latest external collaboration on country level. Dive into details by clicking on the dots.

Hotspot


Dive into the research topics where Russell D. Fernald is active.

Publication


Featured researches published by Russell D. Fernald.


Journal of Computational Biology | 2005

Comprehensive Algorithm for Quantitative Real-Time Polymerase Chain Reaction

Sheng Zhao; Russell D. Fernald

Quantitative real-time polymerase chain reactions (qRT-PCR) have become the method of choice for rapid, sensitive, quantitative comparison of RNA transcript abundance. Useful data from this method depend on fitting data to theoretical curves that allow computation of mRNA levels. Calculating accurate mRNA levels requires important parameters such as reaction efficiency and the fractional cycle number at threshold (CT) to be used; however, many algorithms currently in use estimate these important parameters. Here we describe an objective method for quantifying qRT-PCR results using calculations based on the kinetics of individual PCR reactions without the need of the standard curve, independent of any assumptions or subjective judgments which allow direct calculation of efficiency and CT. We use a four-parameter logistic model to fit the raw fluorescence data as a function of PCR cycles to identify the exponential phase of the reaction. Next, we use a three-parameter simple exponent model to fit the exponential phase using an iterative nonlinear regression algorithm. Within the exponential portion of the curve, our technique automatically identifies candidate regression values using the P-value of regression and then uses a weighted average to compute a final efficiency for quantification. For CT determination, we chose the first positive second derivative maximum from the logistic model. This algorithm provides an objective and noise-resistant method for quantification of qRT-PCR results that is independent of the specific equipment used to perform PCR reactions.


Science | 2008

Genes and social behavior.

Gene E. Robinson; Russell D. Fernald; David F. Clayton

What genes and regulatory sequences contribute to the organization and functioning of neural circuits and molecular pathways in the brain that support social behavior? How does social experience interact with information in the genome to modulate brain activity? Here, we address these questions by highlighting progress that has been made in identifying and understanding two key “vectors of influence” that link genes, the brain, and social behavior: (i) Social information alters gene expression in the brain to influence behavior, and (ii) genetic variation influences brain function and social behavior. We also discuss how evolutionary changes in genomic elements influence social behavior and outline prospects for a systems biology of social behavior.


Nature | 2014

The genomic substrate for adaptive radiation in African cichlid fish

David Brawand; Catherine E. Wagner; Yang I. Li; Milan Malinsky; Irene Keller; Shaohua Fan; Oleg Simakov; Alvin Yu Jin Ng; Zhi Wei Lim; Etienne Bezault; Jason Turner-Maier; Jeremy A. Johnson; Rosa M. Alcazar; Hyun Ji Noh; Pamela Russell; Bronwen Aken; Jessica Alföldi; Chris T. Amemiya; Naoual Azzouzi; Jean-François Baroiller; Frédérique Barloy-Hubler; Aaron M. Berlin; Ryan F. Bloomquist; Karen L. Carleton; Matthew A. Conte; Helena D'Cotta; Orly Eshel; Leslie Gaffney; Francis Galibert; Hugo F. Gante

Cichlid fishes are famous for large, diverse and replicated adaptive radiations in the Great Lakes of East Africa. To understand the molecular mechanisms underlying cichlid phenotypic diversity, we sequenced the genomes and transcriptomes of five lineages of African cichlids: the Nile tilapia (Oreochromis niloticus), an ancestral lineage with low diversity; and four members of the East African lineage: Neolamprologus brichardi/pulcher (older radiation, Lake Tanganyika), Metriaclima zebra (recent radiation, Lake Malawi), Pundamilia nyererei (very recent radiation, Lake Victoria), and Astatotilapia burtoni (riverine species around Lake Tanganyika). We found an excess of gene duplications in the East African lineage compared to tilapia and other teleosts, an abundance of non-coding element divergence, accelerated coding sequence evolution, expression divergence associated with transposable element insertions, and regulation by novel microRNAs. In addition, we analysed sequence data from sixty individuals representing six closely related species from Lake Victoria, and show genome-wide diversifying selection on coding and regulatory variants, some of which were recruited from ancient polymorphisms. We conclude that a number of molecular mechanisms shaped East African cichlid genomes, and that amassing of standing variation during periods of relaxed purifying selection may have been important in facilitating subsequent evolutionary diversification.


Nature | 2007

Fish can infer social rank by observation alone

Logan Grosenick; Tricia S. Clement; Russell D. Fernald

Transitive inference (TI) involves using known relationships to deduce unknown ones (for example, using A > B and B > C to infer A > C), and is thus essential to logical reasoning. First described as a developmental milestone in children, TI has since been reported in nonhuman primates, rats and birds. Still, how animals acquire and represent transitive relationships and why such abilities might have evolved remain open problems. Here we show that male fish (Astatotilapia burtoni) can successfully make inferences on a hierarchy implied by pairwise fights between rival males. These fish learned the implied hierarchy vicariously (as ‘bystanders’), by watching fights between rivals arranged around them in separate tank units. Our findings show that fish use TI when trained on socially relevant stimuli, and that they can make such inferences by using indirect information alone. Further, these bystanders seem to have both spatial and featural representations related to rival abilities, which they can use to make correct inferences depending on what kind of information is available to them. Beyond extending TI to fish and experimentally demonstrating indirect TI learning in animals, these results indicate that a universal mechanism underlying TI is unlikely. Rather, animals probably use multiple domain-specific representations adapted to different social and ecological pressures that they encounter during the course of their natural lives.


Frontiers in Neuroendocrinology | 1999

Gonadotropin-releasing hormone genes: phylogeny, structure, and functions.

Russell D. Fernald; Richard White

Gonadotropin-releasing hormone (GnRH, previously called leutinizing hormone-releasing hormone, LHRH) is the final common signaling molecule used by the brain to regulate reproduction in all vertebrates. Recently, genes encoding two other GnRH forms have been discovered. Here we present a phylogenetic analysis that shows that the GnRH genes fall naturally into three distinct branches, each of which shares not only a molecular signature but also characteristic expression sites in the brain. The GnRH genes appear to have arisen through gene duplication from a single ancestral GnRH whose origin predates vertebrates. Several lines of data support this suggestion, including the fact that all three genes share an identical exonic structure. The existence of three distinct GnRH families suggests a new, natural nomenclature for the genes, and in addition, we present a logical proposal for naming the peptide sequences. The two recently discovered GnRH genes are unusual because they encode decapeptides that are identical in all the species in which they have been found. The control of gene expression also differs among the three gene families as might be expected since they have had separate evolutionary trajectories for perhaps 500 million years.


Animal Behaviour | 1977

Field study of Haplochromis burtoni: Quantitative behavioural observations

Russell D. Fernald; Nancy R. Hirata

Haplochromis burtoni observed in their natural habitat behave differently from those observed in aquaria in several respects. The majority of the territorial males observed in Lake Tanganyika living in a colony did not have the characteristic black eye bar as a part of their bright coloration, although otherwise the colour markings were identical. Those animals with eye bars appeared to be at a disadvantage in competition with barless fish for territories. The non-territorial males, females and juveniles are uniformly cryptically coloured and remained together in two or three large schools on the edge of the colony. These schools drifted slowly through the colony area, attempting to feed in the territories. Territorial males solicited females and chased non-territorial males. Sequences of behavioural events performed during courtship varied, depending on the local population density. Sound recordings showed no purposeful sounds associated with any behavioural acts.


PLOS Biology | 2005

Rapid behavioral and genomic responses to social opportunity.

Sabrina S. Burmeister; Erich D. Jarvis; Russell D. Fernald

From primates to bees, social status regulates reproduction. In the cichlid fish Astatotilapia (Haplochromis) burtoni, subordinate males have reduced fertility and must become dominant to reproduce. This increase in sexual capacity is orchestrated by neurons in the preoptic area, which enlarge in response to dominance and increase expression of gonadotropin-releasing hormone 1 (GnRH1), a peptide critical for reproduction. Using a novel behavioral paradigm, we show for the first time that subordinate males can become dominant within minutes of an opportunity to do so, displaying dramatic changes in body coloration and behavior. We also found that social opportunity induced expression of the immediate-early gene egr-1 in the anterior preoptic area, peaking in regions with high densities of GnRH1 neurons, and not in brain regions that express the related peptides GnRH2 and GnRH3. This genomic response did not occur in stable subordinate or stable dominant males even though stable dominants, like ascending males, displayed dominance behaviors. Moreover, egr-1 in the optic tectum and the cerebellum was similarly induced in all experimental groups, showing that egr-1 induction in the anterior preoptic area of ascending males was specific to this brain region. Because egr-1 codes for a transcription factor important in neural plasticity, induction of egr-1 in the anterior preoptic area by social opportunity could be an early trigger in the molecular cascade that culminates in enhanced fertility and other long-term physiological changes associated with dominance.


Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences | 2008

Expression of arginine vasotocin in distinct preoptic regions is associated with dominant and subordinate behaviour in an African cichlid fish

Anna K. Greenwood; Abigail R. Wark; Russell D. Fernald; Hans A. Hofmann

Neuropeptides have widespread modulatory effects on behaviour and physiology and are associated with phenotypic transitions in a variety of animals. Arginine vasotocin (AVT) is implicated in mediating alternative male phenotypes in teleost fish, but the direction of the association differs among species, with either higher or lower AVT related to more territorial behaviour in different fishes. To clarify the complex relationship between AVT and alternative phenotype, we evaluated AVT expression in an African cichlid in which social status is associated with divergent behaviour and physiology. We compared AVT mRNA expression between territorial and non-territorial (NT) males in both whole brains and microdissected anterior preoptic areas using transcription profiling, and in individual preoptic nuclei using in situ hybridization. These complementary methods revealed that in the posterior preoptic area (gigantocellular nucleus), territorial males exhibit higher levels of AVT expression than NT males. Conversely, in the anterior preoptic area (parvocellular nucleus), AVT expression is lower in territorial males than NT males. We further correlated AVT expression with behavioural and physiological characteristics of social status to gain insight into the divergent functions of individual AVT nuclei. Overall, our findings highlight a complex association between AVT and social behaviour.


Animal Behaviour | 1977

Quantitative behavioural observations of Haplochromis burtoni under semi-natural conditions

Russell D. Fernald

Adult males of the cichlid fish Haplochromis burtoni were observed under semi-natural conditions. Nineteen behaviour patterns were recorded simultaneously with the coloration of the fish. Territorial males spend about one per cent of their time maintaining territories. Their interactions with females follow a fairly regular pattern. The bright body coloration and black head markings are rarely turned off, and then only in specific behavioural situations. Surprisingly, there were no differences observed between the males with and the one without the black eye bar, although this has been shown to be a recognized signal in experiments using dummies. In general, the coloration is closely correlated with the status of the animal and less closely with the immediate behavioural situation.


Science | 2006

Casting a Genetic Light on the Evolution of Eyes

Russell D. Fernald

Light has been exploited for information by organisms through the evolution of photoreceptors and, ultimately, eyes in animals. Only a handful of eye types exist because the physics of light constrains photodetection. In the past few years, genetic tools have revealed several parallel pathways through which light guides behavior and have provided insights into the convergent evolution of eyes. The gene encoding opsin (the primary phototransduction protein) and some developmental genes had very early origins and were recruited repeatedly during eye evolution. Eye lens proteins arose separately and make up a diverse group, many of which were co-opted from other functions. A major challenge now is understanding how newly discovered pathways for processing light evolved and how they collaborate with eyes to harvest information from light.

Collaboration


Dive into the Russell D. Fernald's collaboration.

Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Karen P. Maruska

Louisiana State University

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Barbara I. Evans

Lake Superior State University

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Kim L. Hoke

Colorado State University

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Researchain Logo
Decentralizing Knowledge