Network


Latest external collaboration on country level. Dive into details by clicking on the dots.

Hotspot


Dive into the research topics where Sarah L.H. Ellis is active.

Publication


Featured researches published by Sarah L.H. Ellis.


Applied Animal Behaviour Science | 2005

The influence of olfactory stimulation on the behaviour of cats housed in a rescue shelter

Sarah L.H. Ellis; Deborah L. Wells

A wide variety of feline species have been shown to gain welfare benefits from the introduction of olfactory stimuli to the captive environment. The effect of this stimulation on the domestic cat, however, has been largely overlooked. This study thus explored the influence of olfactory stimulation on cats housed in a rescue shelter to determine whether it holds any value as a method of enrichment for this species. One hundred and fifty cats were randomly assigned to one of five conditions of olfactory stimulation (control [an odourless cloth]; biologically relevant odour [a cloth impregnated with the scent of rabbit]; biologically non-relevant odours, [a cloth impregnated with lavender, a renowned relaxant, or the scent of catnip, a well known stimulant]). Cats were exposed to the relevant olfactory stimuli for 3 h a day for five consecutive days. Each cat’s behaviour was recorded every 5 min on days one, three and five of olfactory exposure, using instantaneous scan sampling. Overall, cats showed relatively little interest in the cloths, spending just over 6% of the total observation time interacting with these stimuli. However, animals exposed to the catnip-impregnated cloths exhibited significantly more interest in the stimulus than animals exposed to the other cloths, spending an average of 11.14% of the observation time interacting with the objects. Across all experimental conditions, interest in the cloths was significantly lower in the second and third hours of stimulus presentation compared to the first, suggesting habituation. Certain components of the cats’ behavioural repertoire were influenced by olfactory stimulation. Catnip and prey scent encouraged a significantly higher frequency of behaviours indicative of reduced activity (e.g. more time sleeping, less time standing and actively exploring the environment) in comparison to the control condition. Catnip also encouraged play-like behaviour characterised as the ‘catnip response’. Overall, the results suggest that certain odours, notably catnip, may hold potential as environmental enrichment for captive


Journal of Feline Medicine and Surgery | 2009

Environmental enrichment: practical strategies for improving feline welfare.

Sarah L.H. Ellis

Practical relevance The clinical application of evidence-based enrichment strategies for the domestic cat housed in a variety of confined environments, ranging from the veterinary cage to the domestic home, is of particular importance - both in relation to providing opportunity for appropriate feline behaviour, and in the prevention and treatment of behavioural and associated health problems (eg, feline lower urinary tract disease associated with negative emotional states such as generalised anxiety). Environmental enrichment has gained particular relevance in the light of current animal welfare legislation. For example, in the UK, the Animal Welfare Act 2006 stipulates that owners/keepers have a duty of care to their animal(s) that includes allowing the animal to exhibit normal behaviour patterns. Evidence base Research into environmental enrichment as a means of improving animal welfare is still very much in its infancy, particularly in relation to the domestic cat. Thus, evidence-based studies are somewhat sparse and more are needed to validate current recommended enrichment practices. Audience This article aims to assist general veterinary practitioners to recognise how cats respond to confinement, and to understand what constitutes environmental enrichment, to help them implement or advise on appropriate enrichment strategies for cats confined in a hospital cage, home environment (particularly an indoor-only home), or cattery or rescue shelter, based on published evidence to date. MULTIMEDIA Three video recordings showing enrichment strategies being applied with the use of drinking and feeding devices, and play sessions with toys, are included in the online version of this article


Journal of Feline Medicine and Surgery | 2013

AAFP and ISFM Feline Environmental Needs Guidelines

Sarah L.H. Ellis; Ilona Rodan; Hazel C Carney; Sarah Heath; Irene Rochlitz; Lorinda D Shearburn; Eliza Sundahl; Jodi L. Westropp

Guidelines rationale: A cat’s level of comfort with its environment is intrinsically linked to its physical health, emotional wellbeing and behavior. Having a basic understanding of the cat’s species-specific environmental needs and how cats interact with their environment will provide a foundation for addressing these fundamental requirements. Environmental needs: Addressing environmental needs is essential (not optional) for optimum wellbeing of the cat. Environmental needs include those relating not only to the cat’s physical surroundings (indoors or outdoors; in the home environment or at the veterinary practice) but also those affecting social interaction, including responses to human contact. Five ‘pillars’ framework: The authorship panel has organized the Guidelines around five primary concepts (‘pillars’) that provide the framework for a healthy feline environment. Understanding these principles and the unique environmental needs of the cat will help veterinarians, cat owners and care-givers to reduce stress, the incidence of stress-related disorders, and unwanted behavior in their feline patients and pets. The recommendations in the Guidelines apply to all pet cats, regardless of lifestyle.


BMC Veterinary Research | 2014

A critically appraised topic (CAT) to compare the effects of single and multi-cat housing on physiological and behavioural measures of stress in domestic cats in confined environments.

Lauren Finka; Sarah L.H. Ellis; Jenny Stavisky

BackgroundDomestic cats have evolved from solitary, asocial predators and whilst they may display social behaviours, they can still exist as solitary survivors. Over-population and relinquishment of pet cats are ubiquitous problems worldwide, and rehoming centres (also known as rescues/ shelters) aim to ameliorate this by holding cats in confinement for a variable period until a new home is found. The provision of optimal housing for large numbers of cats in close confinement, such as in rehoming centres, is therefore inherently difficult. Under these conditions there is the potential for individuals to develop signs of physical and psychological ill health, and thus experience compromised welfare. Available information regarding housing practices that maximise welfare currently provides conflicting results, and as a consequence there are no unanimous housing recommendations. The aim of this study was therefore to review the evidence on the impact of single housing compared to multi-cat housing on stress in confined cats, as measured by physiological and/or behavioural outcomes. The review was conducted using a Critically Appraised Topic (CAT) format. A systematic search of electronic databases (CAB Abstracts, Zoological Records and Medline) was carried out to identify peer-reviewed literature comparing single and multi-cat housing in confined environments.ResultsA total of 959 papers were initially identified, six of which met sufficient criteria based on their relevance to be included within this review. All of the studies had significant limitations in design and methodology, including a lack of information on how groups were assigned, inconsistent handling and enrichment provision between groups, and lack of information on the socialisation status of cats.ConclusionsWhilst some studies suggested that single housing may be less stressful for cats, others suggested group housing was less stressful. Several other important factors were however identified as potential mediators of stress within the different housing systems, and recommendations based upon these findings are presented.


Journal of Feline Medicine and Surgery | 2013

ISFM Guidelines on population management and welfare of unowned domestic cats (Felis catus)

Andrew H Sparkes; Claire Bessant; Kevin Cope; Sarah L.H. Ellis; Lauren Finka; Vicky Halls; Karen Hiestand; Kim Horsford; Christopher Laurence; Ian MacFarlaine; Peter F. Neville; Jenny Stavisky; James Yeates

Guidelines rationale: Cats are among the most commonly kept domestic pets, and coexist with humans in a variety of different circumstances. Cats are sentient beings and, as such, humans have a responsibility for cat welfare where humans and cats coexist. Because cats reproduce efficiently, measures to control populations are frequently needed, but these should be based on ethical and humane approaches. Framework: These consensus guidelines from the International Society of Feline Medicine’s Welfare Advisory Panel provide a framework for the approach to welfare and population control measures, primarily among unowned cats and those going through a homing programme.


Anthrozoos | 2015

Human Classification of Context-Related Vocalizations Emitted by Familiar And Unfamiliar Domestic Cats: An Exploratory Study

Sarah L.H. Ellis; Victoria Swindell; Oliver Burman

ABSTRACT Previous research has shown that human classification of contextspecific domestic cat “meow” vocalizations is relatively poor, although improves with experience and/or general affinity to cats. To investigate whether such classification further improves when recipients (humans) of the vocalizations reside with the vocalizing animal (cat), cat owners (n=10) were asked to listen to eight audio recordings of a single meow (4 from their own cat and 4 from an unfamiliar cat) produced during one of four possible contexts, and identify the context in which each meow was emitted. Contexts comprised food preparation, food-withholding, negotiating a barrier, and attention solicitation. In addition, participants were asked to rate 20 meow vocalizations (produced by unfamiliar cats in the four contexts) on scales measuring pleasantness and urgency, in order to investigate whether participants reached consensus on the emotional content of the vocalizations. Successful identification of both the context and the hypothesized emotional content of the vocalization would comprise the first steps in our understanding of whether human-directed cat “meow” vocalizations are fully advantageous. Forty percent of the participants identified the correct contexts at a level greater than chance when the vocalizations belonged to their own cat. However, no participants performed above chance when vocalizations belonged to an unfamiliar cat. Participants’ urgency ratings were not significantly influenced by the context in which the vocalization was produced. Pleasantness ratings, however, were significantly higher for the context of negotiating a barrier in comparison with attention solicitation. These results suggest that the domestic cat, as a species, does not have a context-specific repertoire of human-directed vocalizations. Successful context classification of cat meows however, was possible for some owners. Whether such success was due to individual learning ability or recognition of hypothesized emotional content of the call requires further investigation.


Animal Cognition | 2016

A reappraisal of successive negative contrast in two populations of domestic dogs

Stefanie Riemer; Sarah L.H. Ellis; Sian Ryan; Hannah Thompson; Oliver Burman

When an anticipated food reward is unexpectedly reduced in quality or quantity, many mammals show a successive negative contrast (SNC) effect, i.e. a reduction in instrumental or consummatory responses below the level shown by control animals that have only ever received the lower-value reward. SNC effects are believed to reflect an aversive emotional state, caused by the discrepancy between the expected and the actual reward. Furthermore, how animals respond to such discrepancy has been suggested to be a sign of animals’ background mood state. However, the occurrence and interpretation of SNC effects are not unequivocal, and there is a relative lack of studies conducted outside of laboratory conditions. Here, we tested two populations of domestic dogs (24 owned pet dogs and 21 dogs from rescue kennels) in a SNC paradigm following the methodology by Bentosela et al. (J Comp Psychol 123:125–130, 2009), using a design that allowed a within-, as well as a between-, subjects analysis. We found no evidence of a SNC effect in either population using a within- or between-subjects design. Indeed, the within-subjects analysis revealed a reverse SNC effect, with subjects in the shifted condition showing a significantly higher level of response, even after they received an unexpected reduction in reward quality. Using a within-, rather than a between-, subjects design may be beneficial in studies of SNC due to higher sensitivity and statistical power; however, order effects on subject performance need to be considered. These results suggest that this particular SNC paradigm may not be sufficiently robust to replicate easily in a range of environmental contexts and populations.


Journal of Feline Medicine and Surgery | 2018

Recognising and assessing feline emotions during the consultation: History, body language and behaviour:

Sarah L.H. Ellis

Practical relevance: Crucial to successful treatment of problem behaviour and optimising the welfare of the individual cat is determining which underpinning emotion(s) are involved in the presentation of the behaviour. Feline emotions are not feelings per se, but motivational-emotional systems that are responsible for instinctual emotional arousal. Often different interventions are required to alleviate different negative emotional motivations. Clinical challenges: Identifying different emotional motivations and the arousal level associated with them solely from observations of behaviour and body language is a difficult task because, as with any species, the behavioural repertoire of the domestic cat is finite and the same behaviour may occur with the activation of different emotional systems. In addition, cats, like people, may experience more than one emotion at the same time or switch quickly between emotional motivations, and this further complicates identification. The behavioural assessment of pain is also notoriously difficult in cats. Evidence base: This review draws on the published literature where available and, where there is a paucity of research, on hypotheses derived from observations of professionals in the field. Global importance: Being able to recognise and assess feline emotional motivations in order to address problem behaviours and improve welfare is important for all veterinarians who see cats.


privacy security risk and trust | 2011

I Can Haz Emoshuns?--Understanding Anthropomorphosis of Cats among Internet Users

Derek Foster; Ben Kirman; Conor Linehan; Shaun W. Lawson; Daniel Mills; Sarah L.H. Ellis; Helen Zulch

The attribution of human-like traits to non-human animals, termed anthropomorphism, can lead to misunderstandings of animal behaviour, which can result in risks to both human and animal well being and welfare. In this paper, we, during an inter-disciplinary collaboration between social computing and animal behaviour researchers, investigated whether a simple image-tagging application could improve the understanding of how people ascribe intentions and emotions to the behaviour of their domestic cats. A web-based application, Tag puss, was developed to present casual users with photographs drawn from a database of 1631 images of domestic cats and asked them to ascribe an emotion to the cat portrayed in the image. Over five thousand people actively participated in the study in the space of four weeks, generating over 50,000 tags. Results indicate Tag puss can be used to identify cat behaviours that lay-people find difficult to distinguish. This highlights further expert scientific exploration that focuses on educating cat owners to identify possible problems with their cats welfare.


Journal of Feline Medicine and Surgery | 2013

Framework for a healthy feline environment

Ilona Rodan; Sarah L.H. Ellis

We are honored to present in this issue the Feline Environmental Needs Guidelines,1 developed through the joint efforts of the American Association of Feline Practitioners and the International Society of Feline Medicine, and most recently endorsed by the American Animal Hospital Association. The Guidelines address the needs of pet cats in any environment, including home, veterinary hospital and shelter. Signs of stress in cats are often subtle and, therefore, difficult to recognise. But cats do indeed experience stress and this is frequently caused by their needs not being met, usually inadvertently because of a lack of understanding of those needs, which differ vastly from our own. Unfortunately, this has resulted in an increase in stress-associated disease such as lower urinary tract disease and obesityassociated diabetes mellitus. It is a sad fact that, in the United States, 58% of cats are overweight or obese,2 and there was a 16% increase in feline diabetes mellitus between 2006 and 2010.3 Additionally, when cats’ needs are not met, we see an increase in abnormal behavior, or normal behavior considered undesirable by the owner. Behavior problems are a common cause of the breakdown of the bond between owner and pet, and the surrender and euthanasia of pet cats.4,5 It is vitally important, therefore, to meet cats’ needs and allow them to express their natural behaviors, to prevent stress and undesirable behavior, and to improve feline health and welfare. The Guidelines are organized around five primary concepts (‘pillars’) that provide the framework for a healthy feline environment regardless of the cat’s lifestyle. Tips for applying the Guidelines are also provided. Panelists from the US and UK were selected to offer different perspectives, and address the needs of both the indoor and indoor/outdoor pet cat. The wide range of expertise included that of two veterinary behaviorists, a scientist specializing in feline behavior and welfare, an internist, two practitioners in companion animal practice, and two in feline practice. Huge thanks go to our co-panelists for all their efforts in making these important guidelines available to all veterinarians. We also appreciate the support of Ed Kanara, facilitator, and Mark Dana for their wisdom and help with the compilation of these Guidelines over the past several months. Incorporating these Guidelines into your practice and client education can help reduce unwanted behaviors, illness and feline stress, and enhance the relationship that cat lovers have with their cats and with your practice. Incorporating the Guidelines into shelters increases feline adoption rates by providing an environment where cats are more active and less fearful. It’s as fundamental as that.

Collaboration


Dive into the Sarah L.H. Ellis's collaboration.

Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Deborah L. Wells

Queen's University Belfast

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Jenny Stavisky

University of Nottingham

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Researchain Logo
Decentralizing Knowledge