Network


Latest external collaboration on country level. Dive into details by clicking on the dots.

Hotspot


Dive into the research topics where Shigeru Kitazawa is active.

Publication


Featured researches published by Shigeru Kitazawa.


Nature Neuroscience | 2001

Reversal of subjective temporal order due to arm crossing

Shinya Yamamoto; Shigeru Kitazawa

How does the brain order successive events? Here we studied whether temporal order of two stimuli delivered in rapid succession, one to each hand, is determined before or after the stimuli are localized in space. When their arms were crossed, subjects could accurately report the temporal order, even when the interval between stimuli was as short as 70 ms. In most trials, subjects could also judge temporal order when their arms were crossed, but only if given adequate time (>1 s). At moderately short intervals (<300 ms), crossing the arms caused misreporting (that is, inverting) of the temporal order. Thus, at these intervals, the determining factor of temporal order was the spatial location of the hands. We suggest that it is not until the spatial locations of the hands are taken into account that the cutaneous signals from the respective hands are ordered in time.


Nature | 1998

Cerebellar complex spikes encode both destinations and errors in arm movements

Shigeru Kitazawa; Tatsuya Kimura; Ping-Bo Yin

Purkinje cells of the cerebellum discharge complex spikes, named after the complexity of their waveforms, with a frequency of ∼1 Hz during arm movements. Despite the low frequency of firing, complex spikes have been proposed to contribute to the initiation of arm movements, or to the gradual improvement of motor skills,,. Here we recorded the activity of Purkinje cells fromthe hemisphere of cerebellar lobules IV–VI while trained monkeys made short-lasting reaching movements (of ∼200 milliseconds in duration) to touch a visual target that appeared at a random location on a tangent screen. We examined the relationship between complex-spike discharges and the absolute touch position, and between complex-spike discharges and relative errors in touching the screen. We used information theory to show that the complex spikes occurring at the beginning of the reach movement encode the absolute destination of the reach, and the complex spikes occurring at the end of the short-lasting movements encode the relative errors. Thus, complex spikes convey multiple types of information, consistent with the idea that they contribute both to the generation of movements and to the gradual, long-term improvement of these movements.


NeuroImage | 2011

Influence of skin blood flow on near-infrared spectroscopy signals measured on the forehead during a verbal fluency task.

Toshimitsu Takahashi; Yoriko Takikawa; Reiko Kawagoe; Satoshi Shibuya; Takayuki Iwano; Shigeru Kitazawa

Brain activity during a verbal fluency task (VFT) has been the target of many functional imaging studies. Most studies using near-infrared spectroscopy (NIRS) have reported major activation in the frontal pole, but those using PET or fMRI have not. This led us to hypothesize that changes in the NIRS signals measured in the forehead during VFT were due to changes in skin blood flow. To test this hypothesis, we measured NIRS signals and the Doppler tissue blood flow signals in the foreheads of 50 participants. The measurements were performed while each participant produced words during two 60-s periods with an interval of 100 s. In addition to a conventional optode separation distance of 30 mm (FAR channels), we used a short distance--5mm (NEAR channels)--to measure NIRS signals that originated exclusively from surface tissues. The oxygenated hemoglobin (oxyHb) concentration in the FAR and NEAR channels, as well as the Doppler blood flow signal, increased in a similar manner during the two periods of word production; the signal increase in the first period was twice as high as that in the second period. Accordingly, the mean changes in oxyHb concentration in the FAR channels were correlated closely with the changes in the NEAR channels (R(2) = 0.91) and with the integrated Doppler skin blood flow signal (R(2) = 0.94). Furthermore, task-related NIRS responses disappeared when we blocked skin blood flows by pressing a small area that covered a pair of optodes. Additionally, changes in the FAR channel signals were correlated closely with the magnitude of pulsatile waves in the Doppler signal (R(2) = 0.92), but these signals were not highly correlated with the pulse rate (R(2) = 0.43). These results suggest that a major part of the task-related changes in the oxyHb concentration in the forehead is due to task-related changes in the skin blood flow, which is under different autonomic control than heart rate.


Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences | 2010

Atypical gaze patterns in children and adults with autism spectrum disorders dissociated from developmental changes in gaze behaviour

Tamami Nakano; Kyoko Tanaka; Yuuki Endo; Yui Yamane; Takahiro Yamamoto; Yoshiaki Nakano; Haruhisa Ohta; Nobumasa Kato; Shigeru Kitazawa

Eye tracking has been used to investigate gaze behaviours in individuals with autism spectrum disorder (ASD). However, traditional analysis has yet to find behavioural characteristics shared by both children and adults with ASD. To distinguish core ASD gaze behaviours from those that change with development, we examined temporo-spatial gaze patterns in children and adults with and without ASD while they viewed video clips. We summarized the gaze patterns of 104 participants using multidimensional scaling so that participants with similar gaze patterns would cluster together in a two-dimensional plane. Control participants clustered in the centre, reflecting a standard gaze behaviour, whereas participants with ASD were distributed around the periphery. Moreover, children and adults were separated on the plane, thereby showing a clear effect of development on gaze behaviours. Post hoc frame-by-frame analyses revealed the following findings: (i) both ASD groups shifted their gaze away from a speaker earlier than the control groups; (ii) both ASD groups showed a particular preference for letters; and (iii) typical infants preferred to watch the mouth rather than the eyes during speech, a preference that reversed with development. These results highlight the importance of taking the effect of development into account when addressing gaze behaviours characteristic of ASD.


Nature Neuroscience | 2001

Sensation at the tips of invisible tools

Shinya Yamamoto; Shigeru Kitazawa

When we touch something with a tool, we feel the touch at the tip of the tool, rather than at the hand that holds the tool. Here we show that the judgment of the temporal order of two successive stimuli, delivered to the tips of sticks held in each hand, was dramatically altered by crossing the sticks without changing the positions of the hands, where the actual mechanoreceptors are located. This provides experimental evidence for the referral of tactile signals to the tip of a tool in the hand.


Journal of Neuroscience Methods | 2010

A rotarod test for evaluation of motor skill learning.

Hiromi Shiotsuki; Kenji Yoshimi; Yasushi Shimo; Manabu Funayama; Yukio Takamatsu; Kazutaka Ikeda; Ryosuke Takahashi; Shigeru Kitazawa; Nobutaka Hattori

The rotarod test is widely used to evaluate the motor coordination of rodents, and is especially sensitive in detecting cerebellar dysfunction. However, mice with striatal dopamine depletion show only mild or no motor deficit on the typical accelerating rotarod. This suggests that dopamine-depleted mice are useful as animal models for non-motor symptoms, because the influence of motor deficit is minimum and easy to discriminate from cognitive aspects of the behavioral change. The typical accelerating rotarod test is designed to evaluate maximal motor performance and is not optimized to detect motor skill learning. In an attempt to make the test more selective to motor skill learning rather than maximal gait performance, we modified the rotarod test by using a slowly rotating large drum to obtain a steep learning curve. Furthermore, administration of nomifensine, a dopamine uptake inhibitor, improved the learning. On the other hand, apomorphine, an agonist of dopamine autoreceptor, a dopaminergic toxin, 1-methyl-4-phenyl-1,2,3,6-tetrahydropyridine (MPTP) impaired the learning. These pharmacological profiles fit the involvement of the so-called phasic dopamine neurotransmission. Using our modified procedure, we found impaired learning of Parkin-deficit mice, which has not been detected in typical accelerating rotarod. The modified rotarod test would be useful for evaluation of dopamine involvement in the acquisition of motor skill learning.


Nature Neuroscience | 2006

Bayesian calibration of simultaneity in tactile temporal order judgment

Makoto Miyazaki; Shinya Yamamoto; Sunao Uchida; Shigeru Kitazawa

Human judgment of the temporal order of two sensory signals is liable to change depending on our prior experiences. Previous studies have reported that signals presented at short intervals but in the same order as the most frequently repeated signal are perceived as occurring simultaneously. Here we report opposite perceptual changes that conform* to a Bayesian integration theory in judging the order of two stimuli delivered one to each hand.


The Journal of Neuroscience | 1995

Effects of delayed visual information on the rate and amount of prism adaptation in the human

Shigeru Kitazawa; Takashi Kohno; Takanori Uka

Accurate reaching towards a visual target is initially disturbed when the visual field is displaced by prisms, but recovers with successive trials. To determine how the improvement depends on the visual error signals associated with the motor output, the time course of prism adaptation was studied with delayed visual information on the error. Subjects were trained to reach rapidly at a target on a tangent screen. Vision was always blocked during the movement, and allowed again only after the index finger touched the screen. One experiment consisted of three sets of 30 trials. In the first set, the subject wore no prisms and vision was allowed without delay. In the second, the visual field was displaced by prisms, and vision was available only after a delay period of 0–10,000 msec while the subjects maintained their final pointing position. Initially, the subject misreached the target by about the amount of visual displacement (60 mm). Errors decreased with trials by an amount proportional to the error in the preceding trial. The rate of decrease of error was generally largest when the delay was 0 msec, became significantly smaller when the delay was 50 msec, and showed only gradual change with longer delays. In the third set, the subject wore no prisms and vision was allowed without delay. Initial misreaching in the direction opposite to the visual displacement, reflecting the amount of adaptation in the second set, was generally largest with no delay (median of 46 mm) and significantly smaller with 50 msec and longer delays (17–33 mm).(ABSTRACT TRUNCATED AT 250 WORDS)


Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences | 2009

Synchronization of spontaneous eyeblinks while viewing video stories

Tamami Nakano; Yoshiharu Yamamoto; Keiichi Kitajo; Toshimitsu Takahashi; Shigeru Kitazawa

Blinks are generally suppressed during a task that requires visual attention and tend to occur immediately before or after the task when the timing of its onset and offset are explicitly given. During the viewing of video stories, blinks are expected to occur at explicit breaks such as scene changes. However, given that the scene length is unpredictable, there should also be appropriate timing for blinking within a scene to prevent temporal loss of critical visual information. Here, we show that spontaneous blinks were highly synchronized between and within subjects when they viewed the same short video stories, but were not explicitly tied to the scene breaks. Synchronized blinks occurred during scenes that required less attention such as at the conclusion of an action, during the absence of the main character, during a long shot and during repeated presentations of a similar scene. In contrast, blink synchronization was not observed when subjects viewed a background video or when they listened to a story read aloud. The results suggest that humans share a mechanism for controlling the timing of blinks that searches for an implicit timing that is appropriate to minimize the chance of losing critical information while viewing a stream of visual events.


Neuroscience Research | 2001

Imaging studies on sex differences in the lateralization of language.

Kenji Kansaku; Shigeru Kitazawa

It has been proposed that language is more strongly lateralized in males than in females. Recent imaging studies, however, have yielded a variety of seemingly contradictory observations. Here, we categorize these observations into three groups: (1) studies that employed sub-lexical tasks applicable to nonwords, which found sex-differences in the anterior language areas; (2) studies that employed tasks applicable to real individual words, which reported lateralized activation in both sexes (and thus no sex-differences); and (3) studies that employed passive listening to stories with a global language structure, which found clear sex-differences in the posterior language areas. We suggest that these differences in observations are explained, at least in part, by the amount of time demanded relative to the interhemispheric conduction delay.

Collaboration


Dive into the Shigeru Kitazawa's collaboration.

Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Shinya Yamamoto

National Institute of Advanced Industrial Science and Technology

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Kenji Kansaku

University of Electro-Communications

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Researchain Logo
Decentralizing Knowledge