Susan Snycerski
Western Michigan University
Network
Latest external collaboration on country level. Dive into details by clicking on the dots.
Publication
Featured researches published by Susan Snycerski.
Management Decision | 2013
Fabian Eggers; Sascha Kraus; Mathew Hughes; Sean Laraway; Susan Snycerski
Purpose – The aim of this paper is to empirically investigate how the business orientations of customer orientation (CO) (represented by responsiveness to customers) and entrepreneurial orientation (EO) (represented by proactiveness, innovativeness and risk‐taking) impact the growth of SMEs.Design/methodology/approach – This study uses a quantitative empirical approach, using structural equation modeling with the software package AMOS to analyze the results of 660 surveyed SMEs from Austria.Findings – This analysis reveals that EO is positively related to SME growth but CO shows a negative association with growth. Moreover, this analysis suggests that SMEs grow the most if they exhibit high EO and low CO.Research limitations/implications – This analysis shows that CO, interpreted as a purely responsive and reactive construct, cannot be considered a strategy that leads to sustainable SME growth. If an SME desires growth, EO is needed to fuel these growth aspirations. In spite of these findings however, thi...
Behavioural Processes | 2005
Susan Snycerski; Sean Laraway; Alan Poling
Groups comprising eight rats initially were exposed to response-independent water deliveries, then to conditions under which a lever-press response raised an empty dipper immediately or after a resetting delay of 15, 30, or 45 s. When their performance was compared to that of control animals using a 90% confidence level, six rats in the immediate-reinforcement group met the primary criterion for response acquisition during a single 6-h session; 4, 4, and 3 did so in the 15, 30, and 45 s delay groups, respectively. Similar evidence of acquisition was obtained when a 95% confidence level was used. With a 99% confidence level, however, evidence of acquisition was not compelling. Although these data appear to provide the first demonstration of response acquisition in the absence of handshaping or autoshaping under conditions where the putative reinforcer is both conditioned and delayed, they also demonstrate that whether response acquisition occurs depends, in part, on how it is defined.
The Analysis of Verbal Behavior | 2001
Sean Laraway; Susan Snycerski; Jack Michael; Alan Poling
Behavior-analytic terminology concerning the so-called inhibitory effect of operant antecedents lacks precision. The present paper describes the problem with current nomenclature concerning the effects of antecedent events that reduce operant responding and offers a solution to this problem. The solution consists of adopting a new term, abative, for the effect in question. This paper suggests that the new term has several advantages over terms currently used and that adopting this term will yield a variety of practical and theoretical benefits, including, but not limited to, a more consistent vocabulary to describe antecedent—behavior relations.
Pharmacology, Biochemistry and Behavior | 2008
Sean Laraway; Susan Snycerski; Lisa E. Baker; Alan Poling
The use of gamma-hydroxybutyrate (GHB), a therapeutic agent and recreational drug, has increased since the late 1990s. Researchers have primarily studied GHBs neurochemical, discriminative, and reinforcing effects, but little is known about the drugs effects on learning, memory, or other complex behavioral processes. This study examined the acute and chronic effects of GHB in rats responding under fixed-consecutive-number (FCN) schedules, which assess working memory. Additionally, we examined stimulus control and response effort as modulators of GHBs effects. GHB dose-dependently reduced operant activity and response rates, but tolerance developed to these effects. GHB had no effect on accuracy or efficiency (i.e., working memory). Stimulus control and response effort did not modulate GHBs effects. These results suggest that GHB produced non-selective behavioral disruption but not working memory impairment.
Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology | 2003
Sean Laraway; Susan Snycerski; Alan Poling
This study investigated the possible motivational effects of (+-)3,4-methylenedioxymethamphetamine (MDMA, ecstasy) in water-deprived rats responding under a progressive-ratio 2 schedule of water delivery. Lower doses (1.0 and 1.8 mg/kg i.p.) had inconsistent effects on breakpoints and response rates, whereas higher doses (3.2 and 5.6 mg/kg ip) significantly decreased both response measures relative to vehicle control levels. Increasing the level of water restriction significantly increased both response measures, and decreasing restriction significantly decreased both response measures. This study found no evidence that MDMA increased the reinforcing efficacy of water, although prior findings have suggested that the drug might have such an effect. MDMA-induced changes in motor activity may account for the present results.
Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis | 2003
Sean Laraway; Susan Snycerski; Jack Michael; Alan Poling
Journal of Intellectual Disability Research | 1999
LeeAnn Christian; Susan Snycerski; Nirbhay N. Singh; Alan Poling
Journal of Organizational Behavior Management | 1999
Kimberly Jarema; Susan Snycerski; Susan Bagge; John Austin; Alan Poling
Psychological Record | 2014
Sean Laraway; Susan Snycerski; Ryan Olson; Bernd W. Becker; Alan Poling
Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior | 2004
Susan Snycerski; Sean Laraway; Bradley E. Huitema; Alan Poling