Network


Latest external collaboration on country level. Dive into details by clicking on the dots.

Hotspot


Dive into the research topics where Thomas J. Balkin is active.

Publication


Featured researches published by Thomas J. Balkin.


Journal of Sleep Research | 2003

Patterns of performance degradation and restoration during sleep restriction and subsequent recovery: a sleep dose-response study

Gregory Belenky; Nancy J. Wesensten; David R. Thorne; Maria L. Thomas; Helen C. Sing; Daniel P. Redmond; Michael B. Russo; Thomas J. Balkin

Daytime performance changes were examined during chronic sleep restriction or augmentation and following subsequent recovery sleep. Sixty‐six normal volunteers spent either 3 (n = 18), 5 (n= 16), 7 (n = 16), or 9 h (n = 16) daily time in bed (TIB) for 7 days (restriction/augmentation) followed by 3 days with 8 h daily TIB (recovery). In the 3‐h group, speed (mean and fastest 10% of responses) on the psychomotor vigilance task (PVT) declined, and PVT lapses (reaction times greater than 500 ms) increased steadily across the 7 days of sleep restriction. In the 7‐ and 5‐h groups speed initially declined, then appeared to stabilize at a reduced level; lapses were increased only in the 5‐h group. In the 9‐h group, speed and lapses remained at baseline levels. During recovery, PVT speed in the 7‐ and 5‐h groups (and lapses in the 5‐h group) remained at the stable, but reduced levels seen during the last days of the experimental phase, with no evidence of recovery. Speed and lapses in the 3‐h group recovered rapidly following the first night of recovery sleep; however, recovery was incomplete with speed and lapses stabilizing at a level comparable with the 7‐ and 5‐h groups. Performance in the 9‐h group remained at baseline levels during the recovery phase. These results suggest that the brain adapts to chronic sleep restriction. In mild to moderate sleep restriction this adaptation is sufficient to stabilize performance, although at a reduced level. These adaptive changes are hypothesized to restrict brain operational capacity and to persist for several days after normal sleep duration is restored, delaying recovery.


Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America | 2009

Decoupling of the brain's default mode network during deep sleep

Silvina G. Horovitz; Allen R. Braun; Walter Carr; Dante Picchioni; Thomas J. Balkin; Masaki Fukunaga; Jeff H. Duyn

The recent discovery of a circuit of brain regions that is highly active in the absence of overt behavior has led to a quest for revealing the possible function of this so-called default-mode network (DMN). A very recent study, finding similarities in awake humans and anesthetized primates, has suggested that DMN activity might not simply reflect ongoing conscious mentation but rather a more general form of network dynamics typical of complex systems. Here, by performing functional MRI in humans, it is shown that a natural, sleep-induced reduction of consciousness is reflected in altered correlation between DMN network components, most notably a reduced involvement of frontal cortex. This suggests that DMN may play an important role in the sustenance of conscious awareness.


Human Brain Mapping | 2008

Low frequency BOLD fluctuations during resting wakefulness and light sleep: A simultaneous EEG-fMRI study †

Silvina G. Horovitz; Masaki Fukunaga; Jacco A. de Zwart; Peter van Gelderen; Susan C. Fulton; Thomas J. Balkin; Jeff H. Duyn

Recent blood oxygenation level dependent functional MRI (BOLD fMRI) studies of the human brain have shown that in the absence of external stimuli, activity persists in the form of distinct patterns of temporally correlated signal fluctuations. In this work, we investigated the spontaneous BOLD signal fluctuations during states of reduced consciousness such as drowsiness and sleep. For this purpose, we performed BOLD fMRI on normal subjects during varying levels of consciousness, from resting wakefulness to light (non‐slow wave) sleep. Depth of sleep was determined based on concurrently acquired EEG data. During light sleep, significant increases in the fluctuation level of the BOLD signal were observed in several cortical areas, among which visual cortex was the most significant. Correlations among brain regions involved with the default‐mode network persisted during light sleep. These results suggest that activity in areas such as the default‐mode network and primary sensory cortex, as measured from BOLD fMRI fluctuations, does not require a level of consciousness typical of wakefulness. Hum Brain Mapp, 2008.


Journal of Sleep Research | 2006

Impaired decision making following 49 h of sleep deprivation

William D. S. Killgore; Thomas J. Balkin; Nancy J. Wesensten

Sleep deprivation reduces regional cerebral metabolism within the prefrontal cortex, the brain region most responsible for higher‐order cognitive processes, including judgment and decision making. Accordingly, we hypothesized that two nights of sleep loss would impair decision making quality and lead to increased risk‐taking behavior on the Iowa Gambling Task (IGT), which mimics real‐world decision making under conditions of uncertainty. Thirty‐four healthy participants completed the IGT at rested baseline and again following 49.5 h of sleep deprivation. At baseline, volunteers performed in a manner similar to that seen in most samples of healthy normal individuals, rapidly learning to avoid high‐risk decks and selecting more frequently from advantageous low‐risk decks as the game progressed. After sleep loss, however, volunteers showed a strikingly different pattern of performance. Relative to rested baseline, sleep‐deprived individuals tended to choose more frequently from risky decks as the game progressed, a pattern similar to, though less severe than, previously published reports of patients with lesions to the ventromedial prefrontal cortex. Although risky decision making was not related to participant age when tested at rested baseline, age was negatively correlated with advantageous decision making on the IGT, when tested following sleep deprivation (i.e. older subjects made more risky choices). These findings suggest that cognitive functions known to be mediated by the ventromedial prefrontal cortex, including decision making under conditions of uncertainty, may be particularly vulnerable to sleep loss and that this vulnerability may become more pronounced with increased age.


Journal of Sleep Research | 2005

Performance and alertness effects of caffeine, dextroamphetamine, and modafinil during sleep deprivation

Nancy J. Wesensten; William D. S. Killgore; Thomas J. Balkin

Stimulants may provide short‐term performance and alertness enhancement during sleep loss. Caffeine 600 mg, d‐amphetamine 20 mg, and modafinil 400 mg were compared during 85 h of total sleep deprivation to determine the extent to which the three agents restored performance on simple psychomotor tasks, objective alertness and tasks of executive functions. Forty‐eight healthy young adults remained awake for 85 h. Performance and alertness tests were administered bi‐hourly from 8:00 hours day 2 to 19:00 hours day 5. At 23:50 hours on day 4 (after 64 h awake), subjects ingested placebo, caffeine 600 mg, dextroamphetamine 20 mg, or modafinil 400 mg (n = 12 per group). Performance and alertness testing continued, and probe tasks of executive function were administered intermittently until the recovery sleep period (20:00 hours day 5 to 8:00 hours day 5). Bi‐hourly postrecovery sleep testing occurred from 10:00 hours to 16:00 hours day 6. All three agents improved psychomotor vigilance speed and objectively measured alertness relative to placebo. Drugs did not affect recovery sleep, and postrecovery sleep performance for all drug groups was at presleep deprivation levels. Effects on executive function tasks were mixed, with improvement on some tasks with caffeine and modafinil, and apparent decrements with dextroamphetamine on others. At the doses tested, caffeine, dextroamphetamine, and modafinil are equally effective for approximately 2–4 h in restoring simple psychomotor performance and objective alertness. The duration of these benefits vary in accordance with the different elimination rates of the drugs. Whether caffeine, dextroamphetamine, and modafinil differentially restore executive functions during sleep deprivation remains unclear.


Journal of Sleep Research | 2004

Comparative utility of instruments for monitoring sleepiness- related performance decrements in the operational environment

Thomas J. Balkin; Paul D. Bliese; Gregory Belenky; Helen C. Sing; David R. Thorne; Maria L. Thomas; Daniel P. Redmond; Michael B. Russo; Nancy J. Wesensten

As both military and commercial operations increasingly become continuous, 24‐h‐per‐day enterprises, the likelihood of operator errors or inefficiencies caused by sleep loss and/or circadian desynchrony also increases. Avoidance of such incidents requires the timely application of appropriate interventions – which, in turn, depend on the ability to measure and monitor the performance capacity of individuals in the operational environment. Several factors determine the potential suitability of candidate measures, including their relative sensitivity, reliability, content validity, intrusiveness and cumbersomeness/fieldability. In the present study, the relative sensitivity (defined as the ratio of effect size to 95% confidence interval) of several measures to the effects of sleep loss was compared in a sleep restriction experiment, in which groups were allowed 3, 5, 7, or 9 h time in bed (TIB) across seven consecutive nights. Of the measures compared, the Psychomotor Vigilance Test was among the most sensitive to sleep restriction, was among the most reliable with no evidence of learning over repeated administrations, and possesses characteristics that make it among the most practical for use in the operational environment.


Journal of Sleep Research | 2011

Sleep disorders and work performance: Findings from the 2008 National Sleep Foundation Sleep in America poll

Leslie M. Swanson; J. Todd Arnedt; Mark R. Rosekind; Gregory Belenky; Thomas J. Balkin; Christopher L. Drake

Chronic sleep deprivation is common among workers, and has been associated with negative work outcomes, including absenteeism and occupational accidents. The objective of the present study is to characterize reciprocal relationships between sleep and work. Specifically, we examined how sleep impacts work performance and how work affects sleep in individuals not at‐risk for a sleep disorder; assessed work performance outcomes for individuals at‐risk for sleep disorders, including insomnia, obstructive sleep apnea (OSA) and restless legs syndrome (RLS); and characterized work performance impairments in shift workers (SW) at‐risk for shift work sleep disorders relative to SW and day workers. One‐thousand Americans who work 30 h per week or more were asked questions about employment, work performance and sleep in the National Sleep Foundation’s 2008 Sleep in America telephone poll. Long work hours were associated with shorter sleep times, and shorter sleep times were associated with more work impairments. Thirty‐seven percent of respondents were classified as at‐risk for any sleep disorder. These individuals had more negative work outcomes as compared with those not at‐risk for a sleep disorder. Presenteeism was a significant problem for individuals with insomnia symptoms, OSA and RLS as compared with respondents not at‐risk. These results suggest that long work hours may contribute to chronic sleep loss, which may in turn result in work impairment. Risk for sleep disorders substantially increases the likelihood of negative work outcomes, including occupational accidents, absenteeism and presenteeism.


International Journal of Pharmaceutics | 2002

The rate of absorption and relative bioavailability of caffeine administered in chewing gum versus capsules to normal healthy volunteers

Gary H. Kamimori; Chetan S. Karyekar; Ronald Otterstetter; Donna S. Cox; Thomas J. Balkin; Gregory Belenky; Natalie D. Eddington

OBJECTIVE The purpose of this study was to evaluate the rate of absorption and relative bioavailability of caffeine from a Stay Alert chewing gum and capsule formulation. METHODS This was a double blind, parallel, randomized, seven treatment study. The treatment groups were: 50, 100, and 200 mg gum, 50, 100, and 200 mg capsule, and a placebo. Subjects consisted of 84 (n=12 per group); healthy, non-smoking, males who had abstained from caffeine ingestion for at least 20 h prior to dosing and were randomly assigned to the treatment groups. Blood samples were collected pre-dose and at 5, 15, 25, 35, 45, 55, 65, 90 min and 2, 3, 4, 6, 8, 12, 16 and 29 h post administration. Plasma caffeine levels were analyzed by a validated UV-HPLC method. RESULTS Mean Tmax for the gum groups ranged from 44.2 to 80.4 min as compared with 84.0-120.0 min for the capsule groups. The Tmax, for the pooled data was significantly lower (P<0.05) for the gum groups as compared with the capsule groups. Differences in Tmax were significant for the 200 mg capsule versus 200 mg gum (P<0.05). The mean ka values for the gum group ranged from 3.21 to 3.96 h-1 and for the capsule groups ranged from 1.29 to 2.36 h-1. Relative bioavailability of the gum formulation after the 50, 100 and 200 mg dose was 64, 74 and 77%, respectively. When normalized to the total drug released from the gum (85%), the relative bioavailability of the 50, 100 and 200 mg dose were 75, 87, and 90%, respectively. No statistical differences were found for Cmax and AUCinf for comparisons of the gum and capsule formulations at each dose. Within each dose level, there were no significant formulation related differences in Cmax. No significant differences were observed in the elimination of caffeine after the gum or capsule. CONCLUSIONS The results suggest that the rate of drug absorption from the gum formulation was significantly faster and may indicate absorption via the buccal mucosa. In addition, for the 100 and 200 mg groups, the gum and capsule formulations provide near comparable amounts of caffeine to the systemic circulation. These findings suggest that there may be an earlier onset of pharmacological effects of caffeine delivered as the gum formulation, which is advantageous in situations where the rapid reversal of alertness and performance deficits resulting from sleep loss is desirable.


Journal of Sleep Research | 2002

Does sleep fragmentation impact recuperation? A review and reanalysis.

Nancy J. Wesensten; Thomas J. Balkin; Gregory Belenky

Studies have shown that next‐day performance and alertness are impaired by sleep fragmentation procedures even when total sleep time (TST) is unaffected. Based on these studies it has been hypothesized that both the duration and continuity of sleep determine its recuperative value. This review of the literature suggests that when sleep fragmentation procedures increase the relative amount of stage 1 sleep, next‐day performance and alertness are impaired. Other studies suggest that stage 1 sleep has little or no recuperative value. Total sleep time, however, is typically defined as the sum of time spent in sleep stages 1, 2, 3, 4, and REM. In the present paper it is shown that when stage 1 sleep is excluded from TST, a stronger relationship between TST and subsequent alertness and performance emerges – and the need to invoke ‘sleep continuity’ as a variable that contributes independently to recuperative sleep processes is obviated. In the same way that partial or total sleep deprivation impairs alertness and performance, it is proposed that sleep disruption also impairs alertness and performance by reducing true recuperative sleep time.


Sleep | 2012

Trait-Like Vulnerability to Total and Partial Sleep Loss

Tracy L. Rupp; Nancy J. Wesensten; Thomas J. Balkin

OBJECTIVE To determine the extent to which individual differences in vulnerability to total sleep deprivation also reflect individual differences in vulnerability to multiple nights of sleep restriction. DESIGN Two sleep loss conditions (order counterbalanced) separated by 2 to 4 weeks: (a) total sleep deprivation (TSD) of 2 nights (63 h continuous wakefulness); (b) sleep restriction (SR) of 7 nights of 3 h nightly time in bed (TIB). Both conditions were preceded by 7 in-laboratory nights with 10 h nightly TIB; and followed by 3 recovery nights with 8 h nightly TIB. Measures of cognitive performance (psychomotor vigilance, working memory [1-Back], and mathematical processing), objective alertness, subjective sleepiness, and mood were obtained at regular intervals under both conditions. Intra-class correlation coefficients (ICC) were computed using outcome metrics averaged over the last day (08:00-20:00) of TSD and SR. SETTING Residential sleep/performance testing facility. PARTICIPANTS Nineteen healthy adults (ages 18-39; 11 males, 8 females). INTERVENTIONS 2 nights of TSD and 7 nights SR (3 h nightly TIB). RESULTS volunteers who displayed greater vulnerability to TSD displayed greater vulnerability to SR on cognitive performance tasks (ICC: PVT lapses = 0.89; PVT speed = 0.86; 1-Back = 0.88; mathematical processing = 0.68, Ps < 0.05). In addition, trait-like responsivity to TSD/SR was found for mood variables vigor (ICC = 0.91), fatigue (ICC = 0.73), and happiness (ICC = 0.85) (all Ps < 0.05). CONCLUSION Resilience to sleep loss is a trait-like characteristic that reflects an individuals ability to maintain performance during both types of sleep loss (SR and TSD). Whether the findings extend to sleep schedules other than those investigated here (63 h of TSD and 7 nights of 3 h nightly TIB) will be the focus of future studies.

Collaboration


Dive into the Thomas J. Balkin's collaboration.

Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Nancy J. Wesensten

Walter Reed Army Institute of Research

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Gregory Belenky

Washington State University Spokane

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Gary H. Kamimori

Walter Reed Army Institute of Research

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Daniel P. Redmond

Walter Reed Army Institute of Research

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

David R. Thorne

Walter Reed Army Institute of Research

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Helen C. Sing

Walter Reed Army Institute of Research

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Maria L. Thomas

Walter Reed Army Institute of Research

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Vincent F. Capaldi

Walter Reed Army Institute of Research

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Researchain Logo
Decentralizing Knowledge