Network


Latest external collaboration on country level. Dive into details by clicking on the dots.

Hotspot


Dive into the research topics where Weiwei Dai is active.

Publication


Featured researches published by Weiwei Dai.


Journal of the Neurological Sciences | 2016

Objectifying eye movements during rapid number naming: Methodology for assessment of normative data for the King-Devick test

John-Ross Rizzo; Todd E. Hudson; Weiwei Dai; Ninad Desai; Arash Yousefi; Dhaval Palsana; Ivan W. Selesnick; Laura J. Balcer; Steven L. Galetta; Janet C. Rucker

OBJECTIVE Concussion is a major public health problem and considerable efforts are focused on sideline-based diagnostic testing to guide return-to-play decision-making and clinical care. The King-Devick (K-D) test, a sensitive sideline performance measure for concussion detection, reveals slowed reading times in acutely concussed subjects, as compared to healthy controls; however, the normal behavior of eye movements during the task and deficits underlying the slowing have not been defined. METHODS Twelve healthy control subjects underwent quantitative eye tracking during digitized K-D testing. RESULTS The total K-D reading time was 51.24 (±9.7) seconds. A total of 145 saccades (±15) per subject were generated, with average peak velocity 299.5°/s and average amplitude 8.2°. The average inter-saccadic interval was 248.4 ms. Task-specific horizontal and oblique saccades per subject numbered, respectively, 102 (±10) and 17 (±4). Subjects with the fewest saccades tended to blink more, resulting in a larger amount of missing data; whereas, subjects with the most saccades tended to make extra saccades during line transitions. CONCLUSIONS Establishment of normal and objective ocular motor behavior during the K-D test is a critical first step towards defining the range of deficits underlying abnormal testing in concussion. Further, it sets the groundwork for exploration of K-D correlations with cognitive dysfunction and saccadic paradigms that may reflect specific neuroanatomic deficits in the concussed brain.


Annals of clinical and translational neurology | 2016

Rapid number naming in chronic concussion: eye movements in the King–Devick test

John-Ross Rizzo; Todd E. Hudson; Weiwei Dai; Joel Birkemeier; Rosa M. Pasculli; Ivan W. Selesnick; Laura J. Balcer; Steven L. Galetta; Janet C. Rucker

The King–Devick (KD) test, which is based on rapid number naming speed, is a performance measure that adds vision and eye movement assessments to sideline concussion testing. We performed a laboratory‐based study to characterize ocular motor behavior during the KD test in a patient cohort with chronic concussion to identify features associated with prolonged KD reading times.


ieee signal processing in medicine and biology symposium | 2016

A parametric model for saccadic eye movement

Weiwei Dai; Ivan W. Selesnick; John-Ross Rizzo; Janet C. Rucker; Todd E. Hudson

This paper proposes a parametric model for saccadic waveforms. The model has a small number of parameters, yet it effectively simulates a variety of physiologic saccade properties. In particular, the model reproduces the established relationship between peak saccadic angular velocity and saccadic amplitude (i.e., the saccadic main sequence). The proposed saccadic waveform model can be used in the evaluation and validation of methods for quantitative saccade analysis. For example, we use the proposed saccade model to evaluate four well-known saccade detection algorithms. The comparison indicates the most reliable algorithm is one by Nystrom et al. We further use the proposed saccade model to evaluate the standard technique used for the estimation of peak saccadic angular velocity. The evaluation shows the occurrence of systematic errors. We thus suggest that saccadic angular velocity values determined by the standard technique (low-pass differentiation) should be interpreted and used with caution.


Digital Signal Processing | 2018

Vector minimax concave penalty for sparse representation

Shibin Wang; Xuefeng Chen; Weiwei Dai; Ivan W. Selesnick; Gaigai Cai; Benjamin Cowen

Abstract This paper proposes vector minimax concave (VMC) penalty for sparse representation using tools of Moreau envelope. The VMC penalty is a weighted MC function; by fine tuning the weight of the VMC penalty with given strategy, the VMC regularized least squares problem shares the same global minimizers with the L 0 regularization problem but has fewer local minima. Facilitated by the alternating direction method of multipliers (ADMM), the VMC regularization problem can be tackled as a sequence of convex sub-problems, each of which can be solved fast. Theoretical analysis of ADMM shows that the convergence of solving the VMC regularization problem is guaranteed. We present a series of numerical experiments demonstrating the superior performance of the VMC penalty and the ADMM algorithm in broad applications for sparse representation, including sparse denoising, sparse deconvolution, and missing data estimation.


Brain Injury | 2018

Validity of low-resolution eye-tracking to assess eye movements during a rapid number naming task: performance of the eyetribe eye tracker

Jenelle Raynowska; John-Ross Rizzo; Janet C. Rucker; Weiwei Dai; Joel Birkemeier; Julian Hershowitz; Ivan W. Selesnick; Laura J. Balcer; Steven L. Galetta; Todd E. Hudson

ABSTRACT Objective: To evaluate the performance of the EyeTribe compared to the EyeLink for eye movement recordings during a rapid number naming test in healthy control participants. Background: With the increasing accessibility of portable, economical, video-based eye trackers such as the EyeTribe, there is growing interest in these devices for eye movement recordings, particularly in the domain of sports-related concussion. However, prior to implementation there is a primary need to establish the validity of these devices. One current limitation of portable eye trackers is their sampling rate (30–60 samples per second, or Hz), which is typically well below the benchmarks achieved by their research-grade counterparts (e.g., the EyeLink, which samples at 500–2000 Hz). Methods: We compared video-oculographic measurements made using the EyeTribe with those of the EyeLink during a digitized rapid number naming task (the King-Devick test) in a convenience sample of 30 controls. Results: EyeTribe had loss of signal during recording, and failed to reproduce the typical shape of saccadic main sequence relationships. In addition, EyeTribe data yielded significantly fewer detectable saccades and displayed greater variance of inter-saccadic intervals than the EyeLink system. Conclusion: Caution is advised prior to implementation of low-resolution eye trackers for objective saccade assessment and sideline concussion screening.


Brain Injury | 2018

The effect of linguistic background on rapid number naming: implications for native versus non-native English speakers on sideline-focused concussion assessments

John-Ross Rizzo; Todd E. Hudson; Prin X. Amorapanth; Weiwei Dai; Joel Birkemeier; Rosa M. Pasculli; Kyle Conti; Charles Feinberg; Jan Verstraete; Katie Dempsey; Ivan W. Selesnick; Laura J. Balcer; Steven L. Galetta; Janet C. Rucker

ABSTRACT Objective: To determine if native English speakers (NES) perform differently compared to non-native English speakers (NNES) on a sideline-focused rapid number naming task. A secondary aim was to characterize objective differences in eye movement behaviour between cohorts. Background: The King-Devick (KD) test is a rapid number-naming task in which numbers are read from left-to-right. This performance measure adds vision-based assessment to sideline concussion testing. Reading strategies differ by language. Concussion may also impact language and attention. Both factors may affect test performance. Methods: Twenty-seven healthy NNES and healthy NES performed a computerized KD test under high-resolution video-oculography. NNES also performed a Bilingual Dominance Scale (BDS) questionnaire to weight linguistic preferences (i.e., reliance on non-English language(s)). Results: Inter-saccadic intervals were significantly longer in NNES (346.3 ± 78.3 ms vs. 286.1 ± 49.7 ms, p = 0.001), as were KD test times (54.4 ± 15.1 s vs. 43.8 ± 8.6 s, p = 0.002). Higher BDS scores, reflecting higher native language dominance, were associated with longer inter-saccadic intervals in NNES. Conclusion: These findings have direct implications for the assessment of athlete performance on vision-based and other verbal sideline concussion tests; these results are particularly important given the international scope of sport. Pre-season baseline scores are essential to evaluation in the event of concussion, and performance of sideline tests in the athlete’s native language should be considered to optimize both baseline and post-injury test accuracy.


Journal of Vision | 2017

A nonlinear generalization of the Savitzky-Golay filter and the quantitative analysis of saccades

Weiwei Dai; Ivan W. Selesnick; John-Ross Rizzo; Janet C. Rucker; Todd E. Hudson

The Savitzky-Golay (SG) filter is widely used to smooth and differentiate time series, especially biomedical data. However, time series that exhibit abrupt departures from their typical trends, such as sharp waves or steps, which are of physiological interest, tend to be oversmoothed by the SG filter. Hence, the SG filter tends to systematically underestimate physiological parameters in certain situations. This article proposes a generalization of the SG filter to more accurately track abrupt deviations in time series, leading to more accurate parameter estimates (e.g., peak velocity of saccadic eye movements). The proposed filtering methodology models a time series as the sum of two component time series: a low-frequency time series for which the conventional SG filter is well suited, and a second time series that exhibits instantaneous deviations (e.g., sharp waves, steps, or more generally, discontinuities in a higher order derivative). The generalized SG filter is then applied to the quantitative analysis of saccadic eye movements. It is demonstrated that (a) the conventional SG filter underestimates the peak velocity of saccades, especially those of small amplitude, and (b) the generalized SG filter estimates peak saccadic velocity more accurately than the conventional filter.


Journal of Neuro-ophthalmology | 2017

Rapid Number Naming and Quantitative Eye Movements May Reflect Contact Sport Exposure in a Collegiate Ice Hockey Cohort

Lisena Hasanaj; Sujata P. Thawani; Nikki Webb; Julia Drattell; Liliana Serrano; Rachel Nolan; Jenelle Raynowska; Todd E. Hudson; John-Ross Rizzo; Weiwei Dai; Bryan McComb; Judith D. Goldberg; Janet C. Rucker; Steven L. Galetta; Laura J. Balcer

Background: The King-Devick (K-D) test of rapid number naming is a reliable visual performance measure that is a sensitive sideline indicator of concussion when time scores worsen (lengthen) from preseason baseline. Within cohorts of youth athletes <18 years old, baseline K-D times become faster with increasing age. We determined the relation of rapid number-naming time scores on the K-D test to electronic measurements of saccade performance during preseason baseline assessments in a collegiate ice hockey team cohort. Within this group of young adult athletes, we also sought to examine the potential role for player age in determining baseline scores. Methods: Athletes from a collegiate ice hockey team received preseason baseline testing as part of an ongoing study of rapid rink-side performance measures for concussion. These included the K-D test (spiral-bound cards and tablet computer versions). Participants also performed a laboratory-based version of the K-D test with simultaneous infrared-based video-oculographic recordings using an EyeLink 1000+. This allowed measurement of the temporal and spatial characteristics of eye movements, including saccadic velocity, duration, and intersaccadic interval (ISI). Results: Among 13 male athletes, aged 18–23 years (mean 20.5 ± 1.6 years), prolongation of the ISI (a combined measure of saccade latency and fixation duration) was the measure most associated with slower baseline time scores for the EyeLink-paired K-D (mean 38.2 ± 6.2 seconds, r = 0.88 [95% CI 0.63–0.96], P = 0.0001), the K-D spiral-bound cards (36.6 ± 5.9 seconds, r = 0.60 [95% CI 0.08–0.87], P = 0.03), and K-D computerized tablet version (39.1 ± 5.4 seconds, r = 0.79 [95% CI 0.42–0.93], P = 0.001). In this cohort, older age was a predictor of longer (worse) K-D baseline time performance (age vs EyeLink-paired K-D: r = 0.70 [95% CI 0.24–0.90], P = 0.008; age vs K-D spiral-bound cards: r = 0.57 [95% CI 0.03–0.85], P = 0.04; age vs K-D tablet version: r = 0.59 [95% CI 0.06–0.86], P = 0.03) as well as prolonged ISI (r = 0.62 [95% CI 0.11–0.87], P = 0.02). Slower baseline K-D times were not associated with greater numbers of reported prior concussions. Conclusions: Rapid number-naming performance using the K-D at preseason baseline in this small cohort of collegiate ice hockey players is best correlated with ISI among eye movement-recording measures. Baseline K-D scores notably worsened with increasing age, but not with numbers of prior concussions in this small cohort. While these findings require further investigation by larger studies of contact and noncontact sports athletes, they suggest that duration of contact sports exposure may influence preseason test performance.


Journal of Neurology | 2017

Capturing saccades in multiple sclerosis with a digitized test of rapid number naming

Clotilde Hainline; John-Ross Rizzo; Todd E. Hudson; Weiwei Dai; Joel Birkemeier; Jenelle Raynowska; Rachel Nolan; Lisena Hasanaj; Ivan W. Selesnick; Teresa C. Frohman; Elliot M. Frohman; Steven L. Galetta; Laura J. Balcer; Janet C. Rucker


Neurology | 2016

Visual Performance of Non-Native Versus Native English Speakers on a Sideline Concussion Screen: An Objective Look at Eye Movement Recordings (I13.002)

Katharine Dempsey; Joel Birkemeier; Todd E. Hudson; Weiwei Dai; Ivan W. Selesnick; Lisena Hasanaj; Laura J. Balcer; Steven L. Galetta; Janet C. Rucker; John-Ross Rizzo

Collaboration


Dive into the Weiwei Dai's collaboration.

Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Researchain Logo
Decentralizing Knowledge