Network


Latest external collaboration on country level. Dive into details by clicking on the dots.

Hotspot


Dive into the research topics where Todd E. Hudson is active.

Publication


Featured researches published by Todd E. Hudson.


PLOS Computational Biology | 2008

Optimal compensation for temporal uncertainty in movement planning.

Todd E. Hudson; Laurence T. Maloney; Michael S. Landy

Motor control requires the generation of a precise temporal sequence of control signals sent to the skeletal musculature. We describe an experiment that, for good performance, requires human subjects to plan movements taking into account uncertainty in their movement duration and the increase in that uncertainty with increasing movement duration. We do this by rewarding movements performed within a specified time window, and penalizing slower movements in some conditions and faster movements in others. Our results indicate that subjects compensated for their natural duration-dependent temporal uncertainty as well as an overall increase in temporal uncertainty that was imposed experimentally. Their compensation for temporal uncertainty, both the natural duration-dependent and imposed overall components, was nearly optimal in the sense of maximizing expected gain in the task. The motor system is able to model its temporal uncertainty and compensate for that uncertainty so as to optimize the consequences of movement.


Journal of Neurophysiology | 2012

Motor learning reveals the existence of multiple codes for movement planning.

Todd E. Hudson; Michael S. Landy

Coordinate systems for movement planning are comprised of an anchor point (e.g., retinocentric coordinates) and a representation (encoding) of the desired movement. One of two representations is often assumed: a final-position code describing desired limb endpoint position and a vector code describing movement direction and extent. The existence of movement-planning systems using both representations is controversial. In our experiments, participants completed reaches grouped by target location (providing practice for a final-position code) and the same reaches grouped by movement vector (providing vector-code practice). Target-grouped reaches resulted in the isotropic (circular) distribution of errors predicted for position-coded reaches. The identical reaches grouped by vector resulted in error ellipses aligned with the reach direction, as predicted for vector-coded reaches. Manipulating only recent movement history to provide better learning for one or the other movement code, we provide definitive evidence that both movement representations are used in the identical task.


Journal of Vision | 2012

Adaptation to sensory-motor reflex perturbations is blind to the source of errors.

Todd E. Hudson; Michael S. Landy

In the study of visual-motor control, perhaps the most familiar findings involve adaptation to externally imposed movement errors. Theories of visual-motor adaptation based on optimal information processing suppose that the nervous system identifies the sources of errors to effect the most efficient adaptive response. We report two experiments using a novel perturbation based on stimulating a visually induced reflex in the reaching arm. Unlike adaptation to an external force, our method induces a perturbing reflex within the motor system itself, i.e., perturbing forces are self-generated. This novel method allows a test of the theory that error source information is used to generate an optimal adaptive response. If the self-generated source of the visually induced reflex perturbation is identified, the optimal response will be via reflex gain control. If the source is not identified, a compensatory force should be generated to counteract the reflex. Gain control is the optimal response to reflex perturbation, both because energy cost and movement errors are minimized. Energy is conserved because neither reflex-induced nor compensatory forces are generated. Precision is maximized because endpoint variance is proportional to force production. We find evidence against source-identified adaptation in both experiments, suggesting that sensory-motor information processing is not always optimal.


Physiological Reports | 2015

Motor planning poststroke: impairment in vector‐coded reach plans

John-Ross Rizzo; Todd E. Hudson; Andrew Abdou; Ira G. Rashbaum; Ajax E. George; Preeti Raghavan; Michael S. Landy

Healthy individuals appear to use both vector‐coded reach plans that encode movements in terms of their desired direction and extent, and target‐coded reach plans that encode the desired endpoint position of the effector. We examined whether these vector and target reach‐planning codes are differentially affected after stroke. Participants with stroke and healthy controls made blocks of reaches that were grouped by target location (providing target‐specific practice) and by movement vector (providing vector‐specific practice). Reach accuracy was impaired in the more affected arm after stroke, but not distinguishable for target‐ versus vector‐grouped reaches. Reach velocity and acceleration were not only impaired in both the less and more affected arms poststroke, but also not distinguishable for target‐ versus vector‐grouped reaches. As previously reported in controls, target‐grouped reaches yielded isotropic (circular) error distributions and vector‐grouped reaches yielded error distributions elongated in the direction of the reach. In stroke, the pattern of variability was similar. However, the more affected arm showed less elongated error ellipses for vector‐grouped reaches compared to the less affected arm, particularly in individuals with right‐hemispheric stroke. The results suggest greater impairment to the vector‐coded movement‐planning system after stroke, and have implications for the development of personalized approaches to poststroke rehabilitation: Motor learning may be enhanced by practice that uses the preserved code or, conversely, by retraining the more impaired code to restore function.


Journal of Neuroscience Methods | 2012

Measuring adaptation with a sinusoidal perturbation function

Todd E. Hudson; Michael S. Landy

We examine the possibility that sensory and motor adaptation may be induced via a sinusoidally incremented perturbation. This sinewave adaptation method provides superior data for fitting a parametric model than when using the standard step-function method of perturbation, due to the relative difficulty of fitting a decaying exponential vs. a sinusoid. Using both experimental data and simulations, we demonstrate the difficulty of detecting the presence of motor adaptation using a step-function perturbation, compared to detecting motor adaptation using our sinewave perturbation method.


Vision Research | 2016

Sinusoidal error perturbation reveals multiple coordinate systems for sensorymotor adaptation.

Todd E. Hudson; Michael S. Landy

A coordinate system is composed of an encoding, defining the dimensions of the space, and an origin. We examine the coordinate encoding used to update motor plans during sensory-motor adaptation to center-out reaches. Adaptation is induced using a novel paradigm in which feedback of reach endpoints is perturbed following a sinewave pattern over trials; the perturbed dimensions of the feedback were the axes of a Cartesian coordinate system in one session and a polar coordinate system in another session. For center-out reaches to randomly chosen target locations, reach errors observed at one target will require different corrections at other targets within Cartesian- and polar-coded systems. The sinewave adaptation technique allowed us to simultaneously adapt both dimensions of each coordinate system (x-y, or reach gain and angle), and identify the contributions of each perturbed dimension by adapting each at a distinct temporal frequency. The efficiency of this technique further allowed us to employ perturbations that were a fraction the size normally used, which avoids confounding automatic adaptive processes with deliberate adjustments made in response to obvious experimental manipulations. Subjects independently corrected errors in each coordinate in both sessions, suggesting that the nervous system encodes both a Cartesian- and polar-coordinate-based internal representation for motor adaptation. The gains and phase lags of the adaptive responses are not readily explained by current theories of sensory-motor adaptation.


Journal of Vision | 2015

Sensory-motor adaptation is (mostly) linear

Todd E. Hudson; Jay Lee; Michael S. Landy

UNLABELLEDnSensory-motor adaptation is usually conceived as an automatic process that maintains the calibration between motor plans and movement outcomes. Viewed as a mechanism that monitors disturbances and produces compensatory motor outputs, sensory-motor adaptation can be thought of as a filter. The first question one normally asks regarding filter performance is whether it is linear. We test homogeneity and additivity using a sinusoidal sensory-motor perturbation of reach endpoints (Landy & Hudson, VSS 2012).nnnMETHODSnSubjects made center-out reaches on a tabletop with fixed starting point, and with target direction and distance chosen to fall randomly within an annulus centered on the start position. Feedback was shown on a frontoparallel display. During each reach, only the target was shown. Fingertip endpoint was shown (shifted) on reach completion. The amount of shift was either a single or the sum of two sinewaves (over trials), with a peak shift of never more than 6 mm. Homogeneity was tested by measuring the response to sinewave-perturbed endpoints following a single sinusoidal disturbance with amplitude A and, in a separate session, 2A. As a test of additivity, the adaptive response to two sinewaves (A and B, of different frequencies) measured separately was compared to the response to perturbation using their sum.nnnRESULTSnThe sensory-motor adaptive response to perturbations in (Cartesian) x- and y-dimensions are consistent with linearity, in that the response to A and B sinusoidal perturbations measured in isolation predict the adaptive response to the 2A and the A+B perturbations. By the same criteria, the polar gain dimension also displays linearity. However, the polar angle dimensions displays a small but statistically significant deviation from linearity in its phase response in the additivity condition: its response to the A+B perturbation lags differently than predicted by its response to the A and B perturbations applied separately. Meeting abstract presented at VSS 2015.


/data/revues/18770657/unassign/S1877065716305164/ | 2017

Iconography : Structured Wii protocol for rehabilitation of shoulder impingement syndrome: A pilot study

John-Ross Rizzo; Peter Thai; Edward J. Li; Terence Tung; Todd E. Hudson; Joseph Herrera; Preeti Raghavan


Archive | 2015

Eye Movements Enhancement of the Vestibulo-Ocular Reflex by Prior

Mingxia Zhu; Richard W. Hertle; Dongsheng Yang; Anand C. Joshi; Matthew J. Thurtell; Mark F. Walker; Alessandro Serra; R. John Leigh; Todd E. Hudson; Michael S. Landy


Journal of Vision | 2012

Multiple coordinate frames for reaches revealed through adaptation

Michael S. Landy; Todd E. Hudson

Collaboration


Dive into the Todd E. Hudson's collaboration.

Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Preeti Raghavan

Icahn School of Medicine at Mount Sinai

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Alessandro Serra

Case Western Reserve University

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Anand C. Joshi

Case Western Reserve University

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Andrew Abdou

University of Medicine and Dentistry of New Jersey

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Dongsheng Yang

National Institutes of Health

View shared research outputs
Researchain Logo
Decentralizing Knowledge