William M. Hay
University of North Carolina at Greensboro
Network
Latest external collaboration on country level. Dive into details by clicking on the dots.
Publication
Featured researches published by William M. Hay.
Behavior Therapy | 1977
William M. Hay; Linda Rudin Hay; Rosemery O. Nelson
The relative efficacy of on-task and academic classroom contingencies was compared on the dependent measures of on-task behavior and rate and accuracy of academic performance. Five male elementary shcool students experienced baseline I, the on-task contingency, baseline II, and the academic contingency. Five other male elementary school student experienced baseline I, the academic contingency, baseline II, and the on-task contingency. The academic contingency was the more parsimonious intervention strategy, producing increments in both on-task behaviors and academic achievement. The on-task contingency increased on-task behaviors without concomitant academic improvements.
Behavior Therapy | 1977
William M. Hay; Linda R. Hay; Rosemery O. Nelson
Covert modeling procedures were employed in the treatment of a 48-year-old male with a 30-year history of excessive alcohol consumption, and a 27-year-old female with a 6-year history of obsessive-compulsive behaviors. The covert modeling procedure in the first case consisted of practicing in imagination appropriate responses to excessive drinking stimuli. Throughout the 11-month posthospitalization follow-up period, the subject remained almost totally abstinent and maintained full-time employment. In the second case, a multiple baseline design was used to evaluate the effectiveness of the covert modeling procedure on each of the subjects obsessive-compulsive behaviors. The covert modeling procedure consisted of practicing in imagination appropriate behavior in situations where obsessive-compulsive behaviors had previously been exhibited. At 6-month follow-up, the subject was no longer engaging in the obsessive-compulsive behaviors. The therapeutic value of teaching the covert modeling procedure as a self-control technique was discussed.
Journal of Research in Personality | 1974
Jacquelyn W. Gaebelein; William M. Hay
Abstract Subjects serving as advisors instructed a confederate which shock intensity to deliver to an opponent in a reaction-time competition; subjects could also directly aggress by setting shock for the opponents partner. Vulnerable subjects (shock electrode on) both instigated less aggression, i.e., suggested that less intense shocks be set, and directly aggressed less frequently than nonvulnerable subjects (no shock electrode). Attacked subjects, those for whom the opponents set shock, both instigated more aggression and more frequently set shock than did nonattacked subjects. The relationship between vulnerability and attack was additive for instigative aggression and multiplicative for direct aggression. Both modes of aggression were significantly influenced by the intensity of provocation from the opponents.
Journal of Research in Personality | 1975
Jacquelyn W. Gaebelein; William M. Hay
Abstract Females, assigned to one of four conditions defined in terms of a confederates behavior, suggested which shock intensity the confederate ought to set for an opponent during a reaction time competition, should the opponent lose the trial (had slower reaction time). Confederates either verbally complied or disagreed with suggestions to set high shock, while either actually setting the intensity suggested or setting a lower intensity. Over trials, the opponent became increasingly provocative. Results revealed the main effects and interaction of confederates verbal and actual behaviors, as well as provocativeness of the opponent, significantly influenced the level of shock subjects suggested. Subjects with verbally and behaviorally compliant confederates suggested more intense shock than subjects who encountered any noncompliance.
Covert Conditioning#R##N#Pergamon General Psychology Series, Volume 81 | 1979
William M. Hay; Linda R. Hay; Rosemery O. Nelson
Covert modeling procedures were employed in the treatment of a 48-year-old male with a 30-year history of excessive alcohol consumption, and a 27-year-old female with a 6-year history of obsessive-compulsive behaviors. The covert modeling procedure in the first case consisted of practicing in imagination appropriate responses to excessive drinking stimuli. Throughout the 11-month posthospitalization follow-up period, the subject remained almost totally abstinent and maintained full-time employment. In the second case, a multiple baseline design was used to evaluate the effectiveness of the covert modeling procedure on each of the subjects obsessive-compulsive behaviors. The covert modeling procedure consisted of practicing in imagination appropriate behavior in situations where obsessive-compulsive behaviors had previously been exhibited. At 6-month follow-up, the subject was no longer engaging in the obsessive-compulsive behaviors. The therapeutic value of teaching the covert modeling procedure as a self-control technique was discussed.
Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin | 1974
Jacquelyn W. Gaebelein; William M. Hay
In two studies of third party instigation of aggression Gaebelein (1973a, 1973b) has demonstrated that the behavior of the potential aggressor can significantly influence the aggressiveness of the instigations. That is, a subject, in the role of advisor, suggested that significantly less intense shock be set for an opponent when the subject’s partner, a confederate, refused to set high shocks, as compared to the intensities suggested by a sub-
Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis | 1980
Linda Rudin Hay; Rosemery O. Nelson; William M. Hay
Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis | 1977
Linda Rudin Hay; Rosemery O. Nelson; William M. Hay
Behavior Therapy | 1977
Rosemery O. Nelson; Linda Rudin Hay; William M. Hay
Behavior Therapy | 1977
Rosemery O. Nelson; Linda Rudin Hay; William M. Hay; Christian B. Carstens