Network


Latest external collaboration on country level. Dive into details by clicking on the dots.

Hotspot


Dive into the research topics where Julian C. Partridge is active.

Publication


Featured researches published by Julian C. Partridge.


The American Naturalist | 1999

Plumage Reflectance and the Objective Assessment of Avian Sexual Dichromatism

Innes C. Cuthill; Atd Bennett; Julian C. Partridge; Ej Maier

Assessment of color using human vision (or standards based thereon) is central to tests of many evolutionary hypotheses. Yet fundamental differences in color vision between humans and other animals call this approach into question. Here we use techniques for objectively assessing color patterns that avoid reliance on species‐specific (e.g., human) perception. Reflectance spectra are the invariant features that we expect the animals color cognition to have evolved to extract. We performed multivariate analyses on principal components derived from >2,600 reflectance spectra (300–720 nm) sampled in a stratified random design from different body regions of male and female starlings in breeding plumage. Starlings possess spatially complex plumage patterns and extensive areas of iridescence. Our study revealed previously unnoticed sex differences in plumage coloration and the nature of iridescent and noniridescent sex differences. Sex differences occurred in some body regions but not others, were more pronounced at some wavelengths (both ultraviolet and human visible), and involved differences in mean reflectance and spectral shape. Discriminant analysis based on principal components were sufficient to sex correctly 100% of our sample. If hidden sexual dichromatism is widespread, then it has important implications for classifications of animals as mono‐ or dimorphic and for taxonomic and conservation purposes.


Advances in The Study of Behavior | 2000

Ultraviolet Vision in Birds

Innes C. Cuthill; Julian C. Partridge; Andrew T. D. Bennett; Stuart C. Church; Nathan S. Hart; Sarah Hunt

Publisher Summary Birds can see ultraviolet (UV) light because, unlike humans, their lenses and other ocular media transmit UV, and they possess a class of photoreceptor, which is maximally sensitive to violet or UV light, depending on the species. Birds have a tetrachromatic color space, as compared to the trichromacy of humans. Birds, along with some reptiles and fish, also possess double cones in large numbers and a cone class. This chapter discusses a range of behavioral experiments, from several species, which show that UV information is utilized in behavioral decisions, notably in foraging and signaling. Removal of UV wavelengths affects mate choice even in species that are colorful to humans. These studies emphasize that avian and human color perceptions are different and that the use of human color standards, and even artificial lighting, may produce misleading results. However, genuinely objective measures of color are available, as are, importantly, models for mapping the measured spectra into an avian color space.


Journal of Comparative Physiology A-neuroethology Sensory Neural and Behavioral Physiology | 2000

Visual pigments, oil droplets, ocular media and cone photoreceptor distribution in two species of passerine bird: the blue tit (Parus caeruleus L.) and the blackbird (Turdus merula L.)

Nathan S. Hart; Julian C. Partridge; Innes C. Cuthill; Andrew T. D. Bennett

Abstract The spectral absorption characteristics of the retinal photoreceptors of the blue tit (Parus caeruleus) and blackbird (Turdus merula) were investigated using microspectrophotometry. The retinae of both species contained rods, double cones and four spectrally distinct types of single cone. Whilst the visual pigments and cone oil droplets in the other receptor types are very similar in both species, the wavelength of maximum sensitivity (λmax) of long-wavelength-sensitive single and double cone visual pigment occurs at a shorter wavelength (557 nm) in the blackbird than in the blue tit (563 nm). Oil droplets located in the long-wavelength-sensitivesingle cones of both species cut off wavelengths below 570–573 nm, theoretically shifting cone peak spectral sensitivity some 40 nm towards the long-wavelength end of the spectrum. This raises the possibility that the precise λmax of the long-wavelength-sensitive visual pigment is optimised for the visual function of the double cones. The distribution of cone photoreceptors across the retina, determined using conventional light and fluorescence microscopy, also varies between the two species and may reflect differences in their visual ecology.


Vision Research | 1987

Visual pigment polymorphism in the guppy Poecilia reticulata.

Simon N. Archer; John A. Endler; J. N. Lythgoe; Julian C. Partridge

Visual pigment polymorphism similar to that found in primates is described in the photoreceptors of wild-caught guppies (Poecilia reticulata). Microspectrophotometric examination of retinal cells revealed rod visual pigments with a lambda max close to 503 nm. Classes of cones with lambda max around 410 and 465 nm were found, together with a population of pigments in the 529-579 nm range. It is in these long-wavelength cones that polymorphism occurs. Male guppies are highly polymorphic for body colour and it is possible that the cone polymorphism is related to the appreciation of the different yellow, orange and red carotenoid colour spots that are used in sexual display.


Journal of Comparative Physiology A-neuroethology Sensory Neural and Behavioral Physiology | 1989

The visual ecology of avian cone oil droplets

Julian C. Partridge

SummaryMicrospectrophotometric (msp) measurements were made of retinal oil droplets of 15 species of birds from 5 orders. The droplets were assigned to six categories on the basis of their cut-off wavelengths. Counts of oil droplets from the retinae of different species revealed large variations in the proportions of oil droplets of different categories. Cluster analysis was used to demonstrate relationships between 12 species of birds on the basis of their oil droplet complements. This analysis linked species in ways which were best explained by ecological factors and which only sometimes reflected phylogeny.


Journal of Comparative Physiology A-neuroethology Sensory Neural and Behavioral Physiology | 1994

The ecology of the visual pigments of snappers (Lutjanidae) on the Great Barrier Reef

J. N. Lythgoe; W. R. A. Muntz; Julian C. Partridge; J. Shand; D. McB. Williams

The visual pigments in the retinal photoreceptors of 12 species of snappers of the genus Lutjanus (Teleostei; Perciformes; Lutjanidae) were measured by microspectrophotometry. All the species were caught on the Great Barrier Reef (Australia) but differ in the colour of the water in which they live. Some live in the clear blue water of the outer reef, some in the greener water of the middle and inshore reefs and some in the more heavily stained mangrove and estuarine water. All the species had double cones, each member of the pair containing a different visual pigment. Using Bakers and Smiths (1982) model to predict the spectral distribution of ambient light from chlorophyll and dissolved organic matter it was found that the absorption spectra of the visual pigments in the double cones were close to those that confer the maximum sensitivity in the different water types. Single cones contained a blue or violet-sensitive visual pigment. The visual pigments in the rods showed little variation, their wavelength of maximum absorption always being in the region 489–502 nm.


Progress in Retinal and Eye Research | 1998

The Eyes of Deep-Sea Fish I: Lens Pigmentation, Tapeta and Visual Pigments

R. H. Douglas; Julian C. Partridge; N. J. Marshall

Deep-sea fish, defined as those living below 200 m, inhabit a most unusual photic environment, being exposed to two sources of visible radiation; very dim downwelling sunlight and bioluminescence, both of which are, in most cases, maximal at wavelengths around 450-500 nm. This paper summarises the reflective properties of the ocular tapeta often found in these animals, the pigmentation of their lenses and the absorption characteristics of their visual pigments. Deep-sea tapeta usually appear blue to the human observer, reflecting mainly shortwave radiation. However, reflection in other parts of the spectrum is not uncommon and uneven tapetal distribution across the retina is widespread. Perhaps surprisingly, given the fact that they live in a photon limited environment, the lenses of some deep-sea teleosts are bright yellow, absorbing much of the shortwave part of the spectrum. Such lenses contain a variety of biochemically distinct pigments which most likely serve to enhance the visibility of bioluminescent signals. Of the 195 different visual pigments characterised by either detergent extract or microspectrophotometry in the retinae of deep-sea fishes, ca. 87% have peak absorbances within the range 468-494 nm. Modelling shows that this is most likely an adaptation for the detection of bioluminescence. Around 13% of deep-sea fish have retinae containing more than one visual pigment. Of these, we highlight three genera of stomiid dragonfishes, which uniquely produce far red bioluminescence from suborbital photophores. Using a combination of longwave-shifted visual pigments and in one species (Malacosteus niger) a chlorophyll-related photosensitizer, these fish have evolved extreme red sensitivity enabling them to see their own bioluminescence and giving them a private spectral waveband invisible to other inhabitants of the deep-ocean.


Proceedings of the Royal Society of London series B-biological sciences | 1998

Ultraviolet cues affect the foraging behaviour of blue tits

Stuart C. Church; Andrew T. D. Bennett; Innes C. Cuthill; Julian C. Partridge

The function of avian ultraviolet (UV) vision is only just beginning to be understood. One plausible hypothesis is that UV vision enhances the foraging ability of birds. To test this, we carried out behavioural experiments using wild–caught blue tits foraging for cabbage moth and winter moth caterpillars on natural and artificial backgrounds. The light environment in our experiments was manipulated using either UV–blocking or UV–transmitting filters. We found that the blue tits tended to find the first prey item (out of four) more quickly when UV cues were present. This suggests that UV vision offers benefits to birds when searching for cryptic prey, despite the prey and backgrounds reflecting relatively little UV. Although there was no direct effect of UV on the time taken to find all four prey items in a trial, search performance in the absence of UV wavelengths tended to increase over the course of an experiment. This may reflect changes in the search tactics of the birds. To our knowledge, these are the first data to suggest that birds use UV cues to detect cryptic insect prey, and have implications for our understanding of protective coloration.


Journal of Comparative Physiology A-neuroethology Sensory Neural and Behavioral Physiology | 1988

Visual pigments in the individual rods of deep-sea fishes

Julian C. Partridge; Simon N. Archer; J. N. Lythgoe

SummaryThe visual pigments in the rods of 15 species of deep-sea fish were examined by microspectrophotometry. In 13 species a single visual pigment was found. The λmax of these pigments, which ranged from 475 nm to 488 nm, suggest they give the fish maximum sensitivity to the ambient light in the deep, blue ocean waters where they live. In two species two visual pigments were found in separate rods.Bathylagus bericoides had rhodopsins of λmax 466 nm and 500 nm andMalacocephalus laevis had two rhodopsins of λ max 478 nm and 485 nm. It is noted that the species with two visual pigments tend to be dark in colour and live in deeper, darker, water.


Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences | 1995

The Molecular Basis for the Green-Blue Sensitivity Shift in the Rod Visual Pigments of the European Eel

Simon N. Archer; Andrew J. Hope; Julian C. Partridge

When the European eel matures sexually and migrates back to deep sea breeding grounds the visual pigments in its rod photoreceptors change from being maximally sensitive to green light to being maximally sensitive to blue light. In part, this change in sensitivity is due to a change in the opsin component of the visual pigment molecule. We used hormone injection to induce these developmental changes in a group of eels and from these animals an opsin coding region was cloned and sequenced using cDNA made from retinal mRNA. From the retinae of hormone-injected eels and those not injected with hormones, distinct opsin mRNAs were isolated. These mRNAs encode two rod opsin proteins that are very similar but have significant amino acid substitutions in key positions that are likely to be involved in spectral tuning of the eel green and blue sensitive rod visual pigment molecules.

Collaboration


Dive into the Julian C. Partridge's collaboration.

Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Nathan S. Hart

University of Western Australia

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Shaun P. Collin

University of Western Australia

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Julia Shand

University of Western Australia

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Researchain Logo
Decentralizing Knowledge