Maria A. G. Witek
Royal Academy of Music
Network
Latest external collaboration on country level. Dive into details by clicking on the dots.
Publication
Featured researches published by Maria A. G. Witek.
PLOS ONE | 2014
Maria A. G. Witek; Eric Clarke; Mikkel Wallentin; Morten L. Kringelbach; Peter Vuust
Moving to music is an essential human pleasure particularly related to musical groove. Structurally, music associated with groove is often characterised by rhythmic complexity in the form of syncopation, frequently observed in musical styles such as funk, hip-hop and electronic dance music. Structural complexity has been related to positive affect in music more broadly, but the function of syncopation in eliciting pleasure and body-movement in groove is unknown. Here we report results from a web-based survey which investigated the relationship between syncopation and ratings of wanting to move and experienced pleasure. Participants heard funk drum-breaks with varying degrees of syncopation and audio entropy, and rated the extent to which the drum-breaks made them want to move and how much pleasure they experienced. While entropy was found to be a poor predictor of wanting to move and pleasure, the results showed that medium degrees of syncopation elicited the most desire to move and the most pleasure, particularly for participants who enjoy dancing to music. Hence, there is an inverted U-shaped relationship between syncopation, body-movement and pleasure, and syncopation seems to be an important structural factor in embodied and affective responses to groove.
Frontiers in Psychology | 2014
Peter Vuust; Maria A. G. Witek
Musical rhythm, consisting of apparently abstract intervals of accented temporal events, has a remarkable capacity to move our minds and bodies. How does the cognitive system enable our experiences of rhythmically complex music? In this paper, we describe some common forms of rhythmic complexity in music and propose the theory of predictive coding (PC) as a framework for understanding how rhythm and rhythmic complexity are processed in the brain. We also consider why we feel so compelled by rhythmic tension in music. First, we consider theories of rhythm and meter perception, which provide hierarchical and computational approaches to modeling. Second, we present the theory of PC, which posits a hierarchical organization of brain responses reflecting fundamental, survival-related mechanisms associated with predicting future events. According to this theory, perception and learning is manifested through the brain’s Bayesian minimization of the error between the input to the brain and the brain’s prior expectations. Third, we develop a PC model of musical rhythm, in which rhythm perception is conceptualized as an interaction between what is heard (“rhythm”) and the brain’s anticipatory structuring of music (“meter”). Finally, we review empirical studies of the neural and behavioral effects of syncopation, polyrhythm and groove, and propose how these studies can be seen as special cases of the PC theory. We argue that musical rhythm exploits the brain’s general principles of prediction and propose that pleasure and desire for sensorimotor synchronization from musical rhythm may be a result of such mechanisms.
Experimental Brain Research | 2017
Maria A. G. Witek; Tudor Popescu; Eric Clarke; Mads Hansen; Ivana Konvalinka; Morten L. Kringelbach; Peter Vuust
One of the most immediate and overt ways in which people respond to music is by moving their bodies to the beat. However, the extent to which the rhythmic complexity of groove—specifically its syncopation—contributes to how people spontaneously move to music is largely unexplored. Here, we measured free movements in hand and torso while participants listened to drum-breaks with various degrees of syncopation. We found that drum-breaks with medium degrees of syncopation were associated with the same amount of acceleration and synchronisation as low degrees of syncopation. Participants who enjoyed dancing made more complex movements than those who did not enjoy dancing. While for all participants hand movements accelerated more and were more complex, torso movements were more synchronised to the beat. Overall, movements were mostly synchronised to the main beat and half-beat level, depending on the body-part. We demonstrate that while people do not move or synchronise much to rhythms with high syncopation when dancing spontaneously to music, the relationship between rhythmic complexity and synchronisation is less linear than in simple finger-tapping studies.
Scientific Reports | 2016
L. Gebauer; Maria A. G. Witek; N. C. Hansen; J. Thomas; Ivana Konvalinka; Peter Vuust
The neuropeptide oxytocin has been shown to affect social interaction. Meanwhile, the underlying mechanism remains highly debated. Using an interpersonal finger-tapping paradigm, we investigated whether oxytocin affects the ability to synchronise with and adapt to the behaviour of others. Dyads received either oxytocin or a non-active placebo, intranasally. We show that in conditions where one dyad-member was tapping to another unresponsive dyad-member – i.e. one was following another who was leading/self-pacing – dyads given oxytocin were more synchronised than dyads given placebo. However, there was no effect when following a regular metronome or when both tappers were mutually adapting to each other. Furthermore, relative to their self-paced tapping partners, oxytocin followers were less variable than placebo followers. Our data suggests that oxytocin improves synchronisation to an unresponsive partner’s behaviour through a reduction in tapping-variability. Hence, oxytocin may facilitate social interaction by enhancing sensorimotor predictions supporting interpersonal synchronisation. The study thus provides novel perspectives on how neurobiological processes relate to socio-psychological behaviour and contributes to the growing evidence that synchronisation and prediction are central to social cognition.
Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences | 2018
Peter Vuust; Martin J. Dietz; Maria A. G. Witek; Morten L. Kringelbach
Rhythmic incongruity in the form of syncopation is a prominent feature of many contemporary musical styles. Syncopations afford incongruity between rhythmic patterns and the meter, giving rise to mental models of differently accented isochronous beats. Syncopations occur either in isolation or as part of rhythmic patterns, so‐called grooves. On the basis of the predictive coding framework, we discuss how brain processing of rhythm can be seen as a special case of predictive coding. We present a simple, yet powerful model for how the brain processes rhythmic incongruity: the model for predictive coding of rhythmic incongruity. Our model proposes that a given rhythms syncopation and its metrical uncertainty (precision) is at the heart of how the brain models rhythm and meter based on priors, predictions, and prediction error. Our minimal model can explain prominent features of brain processing of syncopation: why isolated syncopations lead to stronger prediction error in the brains of musicians, as evidenced by larger event‐related potentials to rhythmic incongruity, and why we all experience a stronger urge to move to grooves with a medium level of syncopation compared with low and high levels of syncopation.
PLOS ONE | 2015
Maria A. G. Witek; Eric Clarke; Mikkel Wallentin; Morten L. Kringelbach; Peter Vuust
There are errors in the stimulus indexing and notational transcripts found after further analysis in Supporting Information S1–S4 Figs, S7 Fig, S1 Table, and S2 Text. The authors declare that the errors do not affect the outcome, interpretations or significance of the study findings, and have provided corrected files here.
Physics of Life Reviews | 2015
Maria A. G. Witek; Morten L. Kringelbach; Peter Vuust
The Quartet Theory of Human Emotion (QT) proposed by Koelsch et al. [1] adds to existing affective models, e.g. by directing more attention to emotional contagion, attachment-related and non-goal-directed emotions. Such an approach seems particularly appropriate to modelling musical emotions, and music is indeed a recurring example in the text, used to illustrate the distinct characteristics of the affect systems that are at the centre of the theory. Yet, it would seem important for any theory of emotion to account for basic functions such as prediction and anticipation, which are only briefly mentioned. Here we propose that QT, specifically its focus on emotional contagion, attachmentrelated and non-goal directed emotions, might help generate new ideas about a largely neglected source of emotion – rhythm – a musical property that relies fundamentally on the mechanism of prediction. Musical rhythm is usually defined as patterns of discrete durations which are usually (but not always) perceived in relation to a pulse, i.e. an underlying framework of regularly occurring beats, also called metre. It is the online prediction of this pulse that enables the synchronisation of body-movements in dance, a musical activity that is uniquely human, enjoyed across history and a wide range of cultures. Entrainment – the process by which an oscillating process is coupled and synchronised with another oscillating process – is believed to provide the mechanism for such sensorimotor synchronisation [2]. Perceptual and motor entrainment relies heavily on temporal expectation and prediction in order for successful coupling to occur, and recently researchers have begun to address its affective significance. One hypothesis claims that through shared sense of time, rhythmic entrainment enables the transfer of emotions between music listeners, dancers and performers [3]. In QT, Koelsch et al. mention that such emotional contagion is afforded by music as well as affective prosody, which provide more direct translations of emotion than semantic language. We would like to emphasise that it is the rhythmic aspects and the associated entrainment in music (and likely also affective prosody) that provides the mechanism for such transfers of emotion. Although there is not yet evidence on the neural basis of entrainment-related emotional contagion, some researchers have suggested that the mirror neuron system and the insula could be involved [4].
Music Perception: An Interdisciplinary Journal | 2014
Maria A. G. Witek; Eric Clarke; Morten L. Kringelbach; Peter Vuust
Advances in Experimental Medicine and Biology | 2014
Peter Vuust; Line K. Gebauer; Maria A. G. Witek
ESCOM 2009 : 7th Triennial Conference of European Society for the Cognitive Sciences of Music | 2009
Maria A. G. Witek