Network


Latest external collaboration on country level. Dive into details by clicking on the dots.

Hotspot


Dive into the research topics where Maricela Alarcón is active.

Publication


Featured researches published by Maricela Alarcón.


American Journal of Human Genetics | 2008

Linkage, Association, and Gene-Expression Analyses Identify CNTNAP2 as an Autism-Susceptibility Gene

Maricela Alarcón; Brett S. Abrahams; Jennifer Stone; Jacqueline A. Duvall; Julia V. Perederiy; Jamee M. Bomar; Jonathan Sebat; Michael Wigler; Christa Lese Martin; David H. Ledbetter; Stanley F. Nelson; Rita M. Cantor; Daniel H. Geschwind

Autism is a genetically complex neurodevelopmental syndrome in which language deficits are a core feature. We describe results from two complimentary approaches used to identify risk variants on chromosome 7 that likely contribute to the etiology of autism. A two-stage association study tested 2758 SNPs across a 10 Mb 7q35 language-related autism QTL in AGRE (Autism Genetic Resource Exchange) trios and found significant association with Contactin Associated Protein-Like 2 (CNTNAP2), a strong a priori candidate. Male-only containing families were identified as primarily responsible for this association signal, consistent with the strong male affection bias in ASD and other language-based disorders. Gene-expression analyses in developing human brain further identified CNTNAP2 as enriched in circuits important for language development. Together, these results provide convergent evidence for involvement of CNTNAP2, a Neurexin family member, in autism, and demonstrate a connection between genetic risk for autism and specific brain structures.


The New England Journal of Medicine | 2008

A functional genetic link between distinct developmental language disorders.

Sonja C. Vernes; Dianne F. Newbury; Brett S. Abrahams; Laura Winchester; Jérôme Nicod; Matthias Groszer; Maricela Alarcón; Peter L. Oliver; Kay E. Davies; Daniel H. Geschwind; Anthony P. Monaco; Simon E. Fisher

BACKGROUND Rare mutations affecting the FOXP2 transcription factor cause a monogenic speech and language disorder. We hypothesized that neural pathways downstream of FOXP2 influence more common phenotypes, such as specific language impairment. METHODS We performed genomic screening for regions bound by FOXP2 using chromatin immunoprecipitation, which led us to focus on one particular gene that was a strong candidate for involvement in language impairments. We then tested for associations between single-nucleotide polymorphisms (SNPs) in this gene and language deficits in a well-characterized set of 184 families affected with specific language impairment. RESULTS We found that FOXP2 binds to and dramatically down-regulates CNTNAP2, a gene that encodes a neurexin and is expressed in the developing human cortex. On analyzing CNTNAP2 polymorphisms in children with typical specific language impairment, we detected significant quantitative associations with nonsense-word repetition, a heritable behavioral marker of this disorder (peak association, P=5.0x10(-5) at SNP rs17236239). Intriguingly, this region coincides with one associated with language delays in children with autism. CONCLUSIONS The FOXP2-CNTNAP2 pathway provides a mechanistic link between clinically distinct syndromes involving disrupted language.


American Journal of Human Genetics | 2003

A Genomewide Screen of 345 Families for Autism-Susceptibility Loci

Amanda L. Yonan; Maricela Alarcón; Rong Cheng; Patrik K. E. Magnusson; Sarah J. Spence; Abraham A. Palmer; Adina Grunn; Suh-Hang Hank Juo; Joseph D. Terwilliger; Jianjun Liu; Rita M. Cantor; Daniel H. Geschwind; T. Conrad Gilliam

We previously reported a genomewide scan to identify autism-susceptibility loci in 110 multiplex families, showing suggestive evidence (P <.01) for linkage to autism-spectrum disorders (ASD) on chromosomes 5, 8, 16, 19, and X and showing nominal evidence (P <.05) on several additional chromosomes (2, 3, 4, 10, 11, 12, 15, 18, and 20). In this follow-up analysis we have increased the sample size threefold, while holding the study design constant, so that we now report 345 multiplex families, each with at least two siblings affected with autism or ASD phenotype. Along with 235 new multiplex families, 73 new microsatellite markers were also added in 10 regions, thereby increasing the marker density at these strategic locations from 10 cM to approximately 2 cM and bringing the total number of markers to 408 over the entire genome. Multipoint maximum LOD scores (MLS) obtained from affected-sib-pair analysis of all 345 families yielded suggestive evidence for linkage on chromosomes 17, 5, 11, 4, and 8 (listed in order by MLS) (P <.01). The most significant findings were an MLS of 2.83 (P =.00029) on chromosome 17q, near the serotonin transporter (5-hydroxytryptamine transporter [5-HTT]), and an MLS of 2.54 (P =.00059) on 5p. The present follow-up genome scan, which used a consistent research design across studies and examined the largest ASD sample collection reported to date, gave either equivalent or marginally increased evidence for linkage at several chromosomal regions implicated in our previous scan but eliminated evidence for linkage at other regions.


American Journal of Human Genetics | 2005

Replication of Autism Linkage: Fine-Mapping Peak at 17q21

Rita M. Cantor; Naoko Kono; Jackie A. Duvall; Ana Isabel Alvarez-Retuerto; Jennifer Stone; Maricela Alarcón; Stanley F. Nelson; Daniel H. Geschwind

Autism is a heritable but genetically complex disorder characterized by deficits in language and in reciprocal social interactions, combined with repetitive and stereotypic behaviors. As with many genetically complex disorders, numerous genome scans reveal inconsistent results. A genome scan of 345 families from the Autism Genetic Resource Exchange (AGRE) (AGRE_1), gave the strongest evidence of linkage at 17q11-17q21 in families with no affected females. Here, we report a full-genome scan of an independent sample of 91 AGRE families with 109 affected sibling pairs (AGRE_2) that also shows the strongest evidence of linkage to 17q11-17q21 in families with no affected females. Taken together, these samples provide a replication of linkage to this chromosome region that is, to our knowledge, the first such replication in autism. Fine mapping at 2-centimorgan (cM) intervals in the combined sample of families with no affected females reveals a linkage peak at 66.85 cM, which places this locus at 17q21.


Mental Retardation and Developmental Disabilities Research Reviews | 1996

Genetics of specific reading disability

John C. DeFries; Maricela Alarcón

Results obtained from the Colorado Twin Study of Reading Disability provide compelling evidence for a genetic etiology. For the present report, multiple regression analyses were performed on reading performance data from 186 pairs of identical twins and 138 pairs of same-sex fraternal twins among whom at least one member of each pair has a reading disability. The results indicate that on average, more than half of the reading performance deficit of probands is a consequence of heritable influences. Moreover, individual differences within the affected twin sample are highly heritable, whereas shared environmental influences do not contribute importantly to twin resemblance. The results of these analyses also suggest that the etiology of reading disability may differ from that of individual differences in reading performance. Because the estimate of heritability for reading performance in affected twin pairs is substantially higher than that in a control sample, reading disability may be caused by one or more genes having major effects. These results are highly consistent with our recent report of a putative quantitative trait locus for reading disability in a small region on the short arm of chromosome 6.


Annals of Neurology | 2006

Search for autism loci by combined analysis of Autism Genetic Resource Exchange and Finnish families

Tero Ylisaukko-oja; Maricela Alarcón; Rita M. Cantor; Mari Auranen; Raija Vanhala; Elli Kempas; Lennart von Wendt; Irma Järvelä; Daniel H. Geschwind; Leena Peltonen

Several genome‐wide screens have been performed in autism spectrum disorders resulting in the identification of numerous putative susceptibility loci. Analyses of pooled primary data should result in an increased sample size and the different study samples have a potential to strengthen the evidence for some earlier identified loci, reveal novel loci, and even to provide information of the general significance of the locus. The objective of this study was to search for potential susceptibility loci for autism, which are supported by two independent samples.


Molecular Psychiatry | 2005

Quantitative genome scan and Ordered-Subsets Analysis of autism endophenotypes support language QTLs.

Maricela Alarcón; Amanda L. Yonan; T C Gilliam; Rita M. Cantor; Daniel H. Geschwind

Autism is a neurodevelopmental syndrome with early childhood onset and deficits in three behavioral and cognitive dimensions: language, social skills and repetitive or restrictive behaviors. We hypothesized that using these endophenotypes would provide more power to detect linkage than the diagnosis of autism. Previously, we reported results for a nonparametric quantitative trait locus (QTL) genome scan in 152 families with autism, which revealed a linkage peak related to spoken language on 7q35. Here, we present the results of a nonparametric QTL scan of autism endophenotypes in 291 multiplex families, including the original 152. The strongest evidence for an ‘age at first word’ QTL was on chromosomes 3q at 147 cM (Z=3.10, P<0.001), and 17q at 93 cM (Z=2.84, P=0.002), both represent novel susceptibility loci for autism endophenotypes. There was also support for a previously identified autism peak on chromosome 17 at 43 cM (Z=2.22, P=0.013) with ‘age at first phrase’. The 7q35 language peak was attenuated (Z=2.05, P=0.02) compared with the original finding. To explore the possibility of increased heterogeneity resulting from the addition of 135 families to the sample, we conducted an Ordered-Subsets Analysis on chromosome 7; these results suggest that the 132 autism families with the earliest average age at first word are responsible for the QTL on 7q35. This locus on 7q35 may harbor a gene contributing variability in spoken language that is not uniquely related to language delay in autism.


Annals of Neurology | 2001

Dementia and neurodevelopmental predisposition: Cognitive dysfunction in presymptomatic subjects precedes dementia by decades in frontotemporal dementia

Daniel H. Geschwind; Janik Robidoux; Maricela Alarcón; Bruce L. Miller; Kirk C. Wilhelmsen; Jeffrey L. Cummings; Ziad Nasreddine

Dementia is typically thought of as a disease caused by the process of aging. Few studies have addressed the premorbid neuropsychological alterations in subjects at risk for the disease–an issue of great importance for the understanding and treatment of degenerative dementias. We used knowledge of the mutation carrier status in a family with inherited dementia to address this issue more efficiently than is possible in the general population, or in cases of inherited dementia where the mutational basis is unknown. Standard neuropsychological tests were used to detect evidence of dysfunction in frontal executive systems in 10 presymptomatic subjects with known mutation carrier status in the highly penetrant condition, frontotemporal dementia and parkinsonism linked to chromosome 17. Presymptomatic carriers demonstrated cognitive dysfunction that was not present in 6 nonmutation‐carrying relatives. Strikingly, frontal– executive dysfunction was apparent in some of the youngest mutation carriers many decades prior to the predicted onset of dementia. Thus, this dysfunction may reflect the native cognitive capacities of affected subjects. These results suggest a potentially important neurodevelopmental component to a dementing condition that has been predominantly considered to be a disease of aging; accordingly, this issue warrants study in other families to assess the applicability of these findings.


Journal of Learning Disabilities | 1997

A Twin Study of Mathematics Disability

Maricela Alarcón; John C. DeFries; Jacquelyn Gillis Light; Bruce F. Pennington

Although results obtained from recent twin and adoption studies suggest that individual differences in mathematics performance are due in part to heritable influences, no genetic analysis of mathematics disability (MD) has been previously reported. In this article we present data from the first twin sample ascertained for mathematics deficits (40 identical and 23 same-sex fraternal twin pairs in which at least one member had MD). When mathematics performance data from these twin pairs were subjected to a multiple regression analysis, evidence for a significant genetic etiology was obtained. However, tests for the differential etiology of MD as a function of reading performance level were nonsignificant. Results of this first twin study of MD indicate that the condition is significantly heritable, but data from additional twin pairs will be required to test hypotheses of differential etiology more rigorously.


Behavior Genetics | 1997

Comorbidity of Mathematics and Reading Deficits: Evidence for a Genetic Etiology

Valerie S. Knopik; Maricela Alarcón; John C. DeFries

In order to assess the genetic etiology of the comorbidity of reading and mathematics difficulties, data were ascertained from two samples: (1) 102 identical and 77 same-sex fraternal twin pairs in which at least one member of each pair is reading disabled and (2) 42 identical and 23 same-sex fraternal twin pairs in which at least one member is math disabled. Composite reading and mathematics performance data from each sample were fitted to the basic multiple regression model for the analysis of selected twin data and its bivariate extension. Resulting estimates of bivariate heritability and the genetic correlation between the reading and the mathematics performance measures suggest that the comorbidity between mathematics and reading difficulties is due in part to genetic influences.

Collaboration


Dive into the Maricela Alarcón's collaboration.

Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

John C. DeFries

University of Colorado Boulder

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Rita M. Cantor

University of California

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Jennifer Stone

University of Western Australia

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Researchain Logo
Decentralizing Knowledge