Network


Latest external collaboration on country level. Dive into details by clicking on the dots.

Hotspot


Dive into the research topics where Laura Quintanilla is active.

Publication


Featured researches published by Laura Quintanilla.


Estudios De Psicologia | 2003

Realismo, Animismo y Teoría de la Mente: características culturales y universales del conocimiento mental

Laura Quintanilla; Encarnación Sarriá

Resumen Este estudio pretende evaluar la universalidadde la teoría de la mente en niños de tres culturas—la zapoteca, la española y la regiomontana—y a su vez, comparar sus concepciones sobre el realismo y el animismo, conocimiento que está marcadamente influido por principios o sistemas culturales. Se utilizaron dos tareas de teoría de la mente (la tarea de creencia falsa de primer y de segundo orden de recursividad) para evaluar los rendimientos de los niños en dos edades distintas; y otras dos tareas para valorar el realismo y el an imismo. Los resultados de este estudio revelan el carácter universal de la competencia de la teoría de la mente y su coexistencia con concepciones culturales y particulares sobre lo mental. Se discute la naturaleza de la psicología natural y de la psicología popular.


Culture and Psychology | 2013

The niche of envy: Conceptualization, coping strategies, and the ontogenesis of envy in cultural psychology

Laura Quintanilla; Kristine M. Jensen de López

Envy is the religion of the mediocre. It comforts them, it responds to the worries that gnaw at them and finally it rots their souls, allowing them to justify their meanings and their greed until they believe these to be virtues.—Carlos Ruiz Zafón “The niche of envy” is a cross-disciplinary attempt to capture and understand the complex and self-conscious emotion of envy as unfolded within social relationships and cultural settings. One of our main interests concerns how children come to understand envy in ontogenesis. Accordingly, we review existing theoretical approaches to understanding envy and introduce preliminary data about children’s understanding of envy. This paper consists of three sections. In the first section, we define envy by introducing the conditions and components that form part of it. We emphasize the fact that envy is a complex and embodied emotion, which embraces a triadic relationship, social comparison, and inequality. In this section, we also introduce social conditions that may facilitate envy and its consequences, such as hostility and aggression. The second section deals with coping strategies for envy. Here, we integrate research from different disciplines, e.g., socio-cultural, psychological, and anthropological research. Finally, in the third section, we introduce a cross-cultural and developmental view of how envy is embodied. We briefly address and offer a critique of Klein’s psychoanalytic view and present recent results from our cross-cultural studies of the ontogenesis of understanding envy.


Early Education and Development | 2015

Improving Social Competence Through Emotion Knowledge in 2-Year-Old Children: A Pilot Study

Marta Giménez-Dasí; Marta Fernández-Sánchez; Laura Quintanilla

Research Findings: The goal of this study was to determine the efficacy of an educational intervention program to improve emotion knowledge, emotion regulation, and social competence in 2-year-old Spanish children. This study makes two original contributions because there are no validated education programs for such young children and because it aims to show how an intervention focused on emotion-related aspects affects children’s social competence. The pilot study was conducted on 57 Spanish 2-year-olds divided into an experimental group (n = 38) and a control group (n = 19). The children were assessed for overall development, emotion knowledge, emotion regulation, and social competence before and after the intervention. The program was carried out in the classroom in 30-min weekly sessions over a 6-month period. The findings showed significant progress in emotion knowledge and social competence as well as slight progress in emotion regulation. Practice or Policy: Our results show that emotions should be part of the curriculum starting in early childhood. On the one hand, 2-year-olds can benefit from planned interventions that improve social competence through emotion knowledge. On the other hand, 24 months is developmentally speaking the perfect age to start acquiring emotion knowledge through the simplest components.


Infancia Y Aprendizaje | 2009

'Competencia' social, 'competencia' emocional : una propuesta para intervenir en Educación Infantil

Marta Giménez-Dasí; Laura Quintanilla

Resumen La definición de las competencias sociales y emocionales no ha sido tarea fácil para los psicólogos. Existen varios criterios e indicios que permiten evaluar en qué medida los niños y los adultos se comportan de forma competente. No obstante, la educación formal recibida para alcanzar dichas competencias es, por lo general, muy escasa o nula. Los resultados de los programas clásicos basados en el aprendizaje de técnicas específicas indican que se produce muy poca generalización a contextos naturales y que la duración de los efectos de mejora es muy corta. Existen, sin embargo, otro tipo de intervenciones que apoyándose en la filosofía e incitando a los niños a reflexionar deforma crítica, proporcionan una formación más profunda y resultados más duraderos. Nuestra propuesta defiende un currículo centrado en la mejora de estas competencias durante el segundo ciclo de Educación Infantil (3–6 años) trabajando desde un enfoque basado en el programa Filosofía para Niños pero que también ofrezca la posibilidad de aprender pautas concretas de conducta, cognitivas y de relación con los demás.


Infancia Y Aprendizaje | 2009

Comprensión emocional en el contexto de la envidia en niños zapotecos y españoles de 3 a 5 años: un estudio exploratorio

Laura Quintanilla; Encarnación Sarriá

Resumen El objetivo de este estudio es explorar la comprensión emocional en el contexto de la envidia, en niños de 3 a 5 años, pertenecientes a dos culturas—zapotecos y españoles—, para las cuales el significado y el modo de afrontar la envidia son distintos. Dos tareas fueron diseñadas, una para obtener atribuciones emocionales de un personaje que presenciaba la destrucción de un objeto de su compañero, que previamente había deseado y que no le era posible conseguir, la segunda explora la relación entre emociones, explicaciones y estrategias de solución al conflicto. Los resultados muestran que los niños de ambas culturas, y desde los tres años, atribuyen alegría por el daño ajeno. Sin embargo, el tipo de explicaciones y las estrategias de solución al conflicto varían según la cultura y la edad. Discutimos estos resultados en relación con el papel que ejerce la cultura en el desarrollo de la comprensión emocional.


Infancia Y Aprendizaje | 2014

Toddlers’ understanding of basic emotions: identification, labeling and causality / La comprensión temprana de las emociones básicas: identificación, etiquetado y causalidad

Marta Fernández-Sánchez; Marta Giménez-Dasí; Laura Quintanilla

Abstract The aim of this paper is to explore the early acquisition pattern of the understanding of basic emotions. Many studies indicate that three-year-old children identify emotions such as joy or sadness, but it is not known how this knowledge arises. Fifty-seven boys and girls between 21 and 32 months were assessed using the Brunet-Lezine-R developmental scale (BL-R) (Josse, 1997) and the Affective knowledge Test (AKT) (Denham, 1986). Through this test we evaluated the children’s knowledge of four basic emotions (happiness, sadness, anger and fear) in three of its components (identification, causality and linguistic labeling). In order to track knowledge acquisition longitudinally, a small subgroup of 19 children was evaluated again six months later. The result of the evaluation by emotions and components allows some conclusions: first, the emotion that older children better understand is anger, followed by happiness and sadness. Second, the first component that children manage is the identification of facial expressions. They then progressively incorporate knowledge about typical causality, and finally, the linguistic labels together with some understanding of atypical causality. The results are discussed from a developmental point of view. This typical developmental pattern also allows some educational and clinical implications.


European Journal of Developmental Psychology | 2017

Children’s understanding of depreciation in scenarios of envy and modesty

Laura Quintanilla; Marta Giménez-Dasí

Abstract The aim of this study was to explore children’s understanding of depreciation when being presented with scenarios of envious or modest behavior. 123 Spanish children, six to eight years of age, decided how a character should respond when faced with a situation of envy or modesty. These situations were described in vignettes. The children chose between a response of either depreciation or admiration in the case of envy and between a response of either modesty or immodesty when faced with modest behavior. They were then asked to explain the reasons for their choices. Generally speaking, the results indicated that children rated self-depreciation in scenarios of modesty more highly than they did depreciation of another in situations of envy. An interaction effect showed that eight-year-old chose modest responses more than immodest ones, in contrast to the six-year-old. Conversely, age differences were not observed for the scenarios of envy. We discuss these results in light of the children’s explanations. Furthermore, the acquisition of emotional display rules is discussed, as well as the ability to manage emotions. Also, we discuss the truths and falsehoods of expression within the framework of social pragmatism in the selection of responses in both scenarios.


Infancia Y Aprendizaje | 2015

To be or to have: what types of objects comprise invidious comparison? / Ser o tener: ¿qué se comprende antes en la comparación envidiosa?

Patricia Recio; Laura Quintanilla

Abstract Envy is a negative and self-conscious emotion that emerges from social comparison. Children compare their possessions and skills with others to construct the Self. The aim of this study is to explore what type of objects — tangible and intangible — are more salient in invidious comparison in order to evaluate the intensity of envy and differences found according to age. Participants included children between the ages of three and nine years (N = 236), who were asked to score how an envious character in a story felt upon witnessing the misfortune of the envied person. In one case, the envied object was tangible (i.e., a scooter), in the other, it was intangible (i.e., being chosen due to possessing greater skill). The results indicated that envy is more intense for all of the age groups in the situations related to possessing greater skill rather than those related to possessing material objects. The younger children (3–6 years) showed more intense malicious envy than the older children. We then discuss the results regarding the role of intangible and tangible possessions in invidious comparison during childhood development.


Cultura Y Educacion | 2015

Thinking emotions with two-year-old children: an educational programme to improve emotional knowledge in young preschoolers / Pensando las emociones con niños de dos años: un programa educativo para mejorar el conocimiento emocional en primer ciclo de Educación Infantil

Marta Fernández-Sánchez; Laura Quintanilla; Marta Giménez-Dasí

Abstract This article aims to present preliminary data to validate and show the effectiveness of the educational programme Thinking emotions with a sample of children aged two years. To test its effectiveness, a sample of 57 children was selected. The sample was divided into experimental group (N = 38) and control group (N = 19). Participants were evaluated for overall development (Brunet-Lezine) and emotional understanding through two tests (AKT and TEC) before and after the implementation of the programme in the classroom. The tutors implemented the programme during school hours in weekly 45-minute sessions over six months. The results show significant advances in emotion understanding of children in the experimental group versus the control group. Results are discussed considering the implications of early education on emotions and the consequences on psychological wellbeing.


Infants and Young Children | 2017

Effects of a Dialogue-Based Program to Improve Emotion Knowledge in Spanish Roma Preschoolers.

Marta Giménez-Dasí; Laura Quintanilla; Vanesa Ojeda; Beatriz Lucas-Molina

Romas are one of the largest minority groups in Spain and Europe, but no specific data on childrens socioemotional learning are available. Our goal was to determine the level of socioemotional knowledge of a group of 4- and 5-year-old Roma children and to implement an intervention program at school. Forty-three Roma children participated (experimental: N = 21; control: N = 22). We implemented the intervention program Thinking emotions (M. Giménez-Dasí, M. Fernández-Sánchez, & M.-F. Daniel, 2013a). This program is based on Philosophy for Children (M. Lipman, A. M. Sharp, & F. S. Oscanyan, 1980) and aims to promote emotion knowledge, regulation strategies, and social competencies through peer-to-peer dialogue. Pre- and posttest measures were taken. Results showed low levels of initial knowledge and a clear pattern of improvement after the intervention.

Collaboration


Dive into the Laura Quintanilla's collaboration.

Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Encarnación Sarriá

National University of Distance Education

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Marta Fernández-Sánchez

National University of Distance Education

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Lina Arias

Autonomous University of Madrid

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

María Núñez

Autonomous University of Madrid

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

María Sotillo

Autonomous University of Madrid

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Ángel Rivière

Autonomous University of Madrid

View shared research outputs
Top Co-Authors

Avatar

Alberto Fernández-Angulo

National University of Distance Education

View shared research outputs
Researchain Logo
Decentralizing Knowledge